Causes of Variability. Effects
of Habit. Correlation of Growth. Inheritance.
Character of Domestic Varieties. Difficulty of
distinguishing between Varieties and Species.
Origin of Domestic Varieties from one or more Species.
Domestic Pigeons, their Differences and Origin.
Principle of Selection anciently followed, its Effects.
Methodical and Unconscious Selection. Unknown
Origin of our Domestic Productions. Circumstances
favourable to Man’s power of Selection.
When we look to the individuals of
the same variety or sub-variety of our older cultivated
plants and animals, one of the first points which
strikes us, is, that they generally differ much more
from each other, than do the individuals of any one
species or variety in a state of nature. When
we reflect on the vast diversity of the plants and
animals which have been cultivated, and which have
varied during all ages under the most different climates
and treatment, I think we are driven to conclude that
this greater variability is simply due to our domestic
productions having been raised under conditions of
life not so uniform as, and somewhat different from,
those to which the parent-species have been exposed
under nature. There is, also, I think, some probability
in the view propounded by Andrew Knight, that this
variability may be partly connected with excess of
food. It seems pretty clear that organic beings
must be exposed during several generations to the new
conditions of life to cause any appreciable amount
of variation; and that when the organisation has once
begun to vary, it generally continues to vary for
many generations. No case is on record of a variable
being ceasing to be variable under cultivation.
Our oldest cultivated plants, such as wheat, still
often yield new varieties: our oldest domesticated
animals are still capable of rapid improvement or
modification.
It has been disputed at what period
of life the causes of variability, whatever they may
be, generally act; whether during the early or late
period of development of the embryo, or at the instant
of conception. Geoffroy St. Hilaire’s experiments
show that unnatural treatment of the embryo causes
monstrosities; and monstrosities cannot be separated
by any clear line of distinction from mere variations.
But I am strongly inclined to suspect that the most
frequent cause of variability may be attributed to
the male and female reproductive elements having been
affected prior to the act of conception. Several
reasons make me believe in this; but the chief one
is the remarkable effect which confinement or cultivation
has on the functions of the reproductive system; this
system appearing to be far more susceptible than any
other part of the organisation, to the action of any
change in the conditions of life. Nothing is
more easy than to tame an animal, and few things more
difficult than to get it to breed freely under confinement,
even in the many cases when the male and female unite.
How many animals there are which will not breed, though
living long under not very close confinement in their
native country! This is generally attributed to
vitiated instincts; but how many cultivated plants
display the utmost vigour, and yet rarely or never
seed! In some few such cases it has been found
out that very trifling changes, such as a little more
or less water at some particular period of growth,
will determine whether or not the plant sets a seed.
I cannot here enter on the copious details which I
have collected on this curious subject; but to show
how singular the laws are which determine the reproduction
of animals under confinement, I may just mention that
carnivorous animals, even from the tropics, breed
in this country pretty freely under confinement, with
the exception of the plantigrades or bear family;
whereas, carnivorous birds, with the rarest exceptions,
hardly ever lay fertile eggs. Many exotic plants
have pollen utterly worthless, in the same exact condition
as in the most sterile hybrids. When, on the one
hand, we see domesticated animals and plants, though
often weak and sickly, yet breeding quite freely under
confinement; and when, on the other hand, we see individuals,
though taken young from a state of nature, perfectly
tamed, long-lived, and healthy (of which I could give
numerous instances), yet having their reproductive
system so seriously affected by unperceived causes
as to fail in acting, we need not be surprised at
this system, when it does act under confinement, acting
not quite regularly, and producing offspring not perfectly
like their parents or variable.
Sterility has been said to be the
bane of horticulture; but on this view we owe variability
to the same cause which produces sterility; and variability
is the source of all the choicest productions of the
garden. I may add, that as some organisms will
breed most freely under the most unnatural conditions
(for instance, the rabbit and ferret kept in hutches),
showing that their reproductive system has not been
thus affected; so will some animals and plants withstand
domestication or cultivation, and vary very slightly perhaps
hardly more than in a state of nature.
A long list could easily be given
of “sporting plants;” by this term gardeners
mean a single bud or offset, which suddenly assumes
a new and sometimes very different character from
that of the rest of the plant. Such buds can
be propagated by grafting, etc., and sometimes
by seed. These “sports” are extremely
rare under nature, but far from rare under cultivation;
and in this case we see that the treatment of the parent
has affected a bud or offset, and not the ovules
or pollen. But it is the opinion of most physiologists
that there is no essential difference between a bud
and an ovule in their earliest stages of formation;
so that, in fact, “sports” support my
view, that variability may be largely attributed to
the ovules or pollen, or to both, having been
affected by the treatment of the parent prior to the
act of conception. These cases anyhow show that
variation is not necessarily connected, as some authors
have supposed, with the act of generation.
Seedlings from the same fruit, and
the young of the same litter, sometimes differ considerably
from each other, though both the young and the parents,
as Muller has remarked, have apparently been exposed
to exactly the same conditions of life; and this shows
how unimportant the direct effects of the conditions
of life are in comparison with the laws of reproduction,
and of growth, and of inheritance; for had the action
of the conditions been direct, if any of the young
had varied, all would probably have varied in the
same manner. To judge how much, in the case of
any variation, we should attribute to the direct action
of heat, moisture, light, food, etc., is most
difficult: my impression is, that with animals
such agencies have produced very little direct effect,
though apparently more in the case of plants.
Under this point of view, Mr. Buckman’s recent
experiments on plants seem extremely valuable.
When all or nearly all the individuals exposed to certain
conditions are affected in the same way, the change
at first appears to be directly due to such conditions;
but in some cases it can be shown that quite opposite
conditions produce similar changes of structure.
Nevertheless some slight amount of change may, I think,
be attributed to the direct action of the conditions
of life as, in some cases, increased size
from amount of food, colour from particular kinds
of food and from light, and perhaps the thickness
of fur from climate.
Habit also has a decided influence,
as in the period of flowering with plants when transported
from one climate to another. In animals it has
a more marked effect; for instance, I find in the domestic
duck that the bones of the wing weigh less and the
bones of the leg more, in proportion to the whole
skeleton, than do the same bones in the wild-duck;
and I presume that this change may be safely attributed
to the domestic duck flying much less, and walking
more, than its wild parent. The great and inherited
development of the udders in cows and goats in countries
where they are habitually milked, in comparison with
the state of these organs in other countries, is another
instance of the effect of use. Not a single domestic
animal can be named which has not in some country
drooping ears; and the view suggested by some authors,
that the drooping is due to the disuse of the muscles
of the ear, from the animals not being much alarmed
by danger, seems probable.
There are many laws regulating variation,
some few of which can be dimly seen, and will be hereafter
briefly mentioned. I will here only allude to
what may be called correlation of growth. Any
change in the embryo or larva will almost certainly
entail changes in the mature animal. In monstrosities,
the correlations between quite distinct parts are very
curious; and many instances are given in Isidore Geoffroy
St. Hilaire’s great work on this subject.
Breeders believe that long limbs are almost always
accompanied by an elongated head. Some instances
of correlation are quite whimsical; thus cats with
blue eyes are invariably deaf; colour and constitutional
peculiarities go together, of which many remarkable
cases could be given amongst animals and plants.
From the facts collected by Heusinger, it appears
that white sheep and pigs are differently affected
from coloured individuals by certain vegetable poisons.
Hairless dogs have imperfect teeth; long-haired and
coarse-haired animals are apt to have, as is asserted,
long or many horns; pigeons with feathered feet have
skin between their outer toes; pigeons with short
beaks have small feet, and those with long beaks large
feet. Hence, if man goes on selecting, and thus
augmenting, any peculiarity, he will almost certainly
unconsciously modify other parts of the structure,
owing to the mysterious laws of the correlation of
growth.
The result of the various, quite unknown,
or dimly seen laws of variation is infinitely complex
and diversified. It is well worth while carefully
to study the several treatises published on some of
our old cultivated plants, as on the hyacinth, potato,
even the dahlia, etc.; and it is really surprising
to note the endless points in structure and constitution
in which the varieties and sub-varieties differ slightly
from each other. The whole organisation seems
to have become plastic, and tends to depart in some
small degree from that of the parental type.
Any variation which is not inherited
is unimportant for us. But the number and diversity
of inheritable deviations of structure, both those
of slight and those of considerable physiological importance,
is endless. Dr. Prosper Lucas’s treatise,
in two large volumes, is the fullest and the best
on this subject. No breeder doubts how strong
is the tendency to inheritance: like produces
like is his fundamental belief: doubts have been
thrown on this principle by theoretical writers alone.
When a deviation appears not unfrequently, and we see
it in the father and child, we cannot tell whether
it may not be due to the same original cause acting
on both; but when amongst individuals, apparently
exposed to the same conditions, any very rare deviation,
due to some extraordinary combination of circumstances,
appears in the parent say, once amongst
several million individuals and it reappears
in the child, the mere doctrine of chances almost
compels us to attribute its reappearance to inheritance.
Every one must have heard of cases of albinism, prickly
skin, hairy bodies, etc., appearing in several
members of the same family. If strange and rare
deviations of structure are truly inherited, less
strange and commoner deviations may be freely admitted
to be inheritable. Perhaps the correct way of
viewing the whole subject, would be, to look at the
inheritance of every character whatever as the rule,
and non-inheritance as the anomaly.
The laws governing inheritance are
quite unknown; no one can say why the same peculiarity
in different individuals of the same species, and in
individuals of different species, is sometimes inherited
and sometimes not so; why the child often reverts
in certain characters to its grandfather or grandmother
or other much more remote ancestor; why a peculiarity
is often transmitted from one sex to both sexes or
to one sex alone, more commonly but not exclusively
to the like sex. It is a fact of some little
importance to us, that peculiarities appearing in
the males of our domestic breeds are often transmitted
either exclusively, or in a much greater degree, to
males alone. A much more important rule, which
I think may be trusted, is that, at whatever period
of life a peculiarity first appears, it tends to appear
in the offspring at a corresponding age, though sometimes
earlier. In many cases this could not be otherwise:
thus the inherited peculiarities in the horns of cattle
could appear only in the offspring when nearly mature;
peculiarities in the silkworm are known to appear at
the corresponding caterpillar or cocoon stage.
But hereditary diseases and some other facts make
me believe that the rule has a wider extension, and
that when there is no apparent reason why a peculiarity
should appear at any particular age, yet that it does
tend to appear in the offspring at the same period
at which it first appeared in the parent. I believe
this rule to be of the highest importance in explaining
the laws of embryology. These remarks are of
course confined to the first appearance of the
peculiarity, and not to its primary cause, which may
have acted on the ovules or male element; in nearly
the same manner as in the crossed offspring from a
short-horned cow by a long-horned bull, the greater
length of horn, though appearing late in life, is clearly
due to the male element.
Having alluded to the subject of reversion,
I may here refer to a statement often made by naturalists namely,
that our domestic varieties, when run wild, gradually
but certainly revert in character to their aboriginal
stocks. Hence it has been argued that no deductions
can be drawn from domestic races to species in a state
of nature. I have in vain endeavoured to discover
on what decisive facts the above statement has so
often and so boldly been made. There would be
great difficulty in proving its truth: we may
safely conclude that very many of the most strongly-marked
domestic varieties could not possibly live in a wild
state. In many cases we do not know what the aboriginal
stock was, and so could not tell whether or not nearly
perfect reversion had ensued. It would be quite
necessary, in order to prevent the effects of intercrossing,
that only a single variety should be turned loose in
its new home. Nevertheless, as our varieties certainly
do occasionally revert in some of their characters
to ancestral forms, it seems to me not improbable,
that if we could succeed in naturalising, or were to
cultivate, during many generations, the several races,
for instance, of the cabbage, in very poor soil (in
which case, however, some effect would have to be
attributed to the direct action of the poor soil),
that they would to a large extent, or even wholly,
revert to the wild aboriginal stock. Whether
or not the experiment would succeed, is not of great
importance for our line of argument; for by the experiment
itself the conditions of life are changed. If
it could be shown that our domestic varieties manifested
a strong tendency to reversion, that is,
to lose their acquired characters, whilst kept under
unchanged conditions, and whilst kept in a considerable
body, so that free intercrossing might check, by blending
together, any slight deviations of structure, in such
case, I grant that we could deduce nothing from domestic
varieties in regard to species. But there is not
a shadow of evidence in favour of this view:
to assert that we could not breed our cart and race-horses,
long and short-horned cattle, and poultry of various
breeds, and esculent vegetables, for an almost infinite
number of generations, would be opposed to all experience.
I may add, that when under nature the conditions of
life do change, variations and reversions of character
probably do occur; but natural selection, as will hereafter
be explained, will determine how far the new characters
thus arising shall be preserved.
When we look to the hereditary varieties
or races of our domestic animals and plants, and compare
them with species closely allied together, we generally
perceive in each domestic race, as already remarked,
less uniformity of character than in true species.
Domestic races of the same species, also, often have
a somewhat monstrous character; by which I mean, that,
although differing from each other, and from the other
species of the same genus, in several trifling respects,
they often differ in an extreme degree in some one
part, both when compared one with another, and more
especially when compared with all the species in nature
to which they are nearest allied. With these
exceptions (and with that of the perfect fertility
of varieties when crossed, a subject hereafter
to be discussed), domestic races of the same species
differ from each other in the same manner as, only
in most cases in a lesser degree than, do closely-allied
species of the same genus in a state of nature.
I think this must be admitted, when we find that there
are hardly any domestic races, either amongst animals
or plants, which have not been ranked by some competent
judges as mere varieties, and by other competent judges
as the descendants of aboriginally distinct species.
If any marked distinction existed between domestic
races and species, this source of doubt could not so
perpetually recur. It has often been stated that
domestic races do not differ from each other in characters
of generic value. I think it could be shown that
this statement is hardly correct; but naturalists differ
most widely in determining what characters are of generic
value; all such valuations being at present empirical.
Moreover, on the view of the origin of genera which
I shall presently give, we have no right to expect
often to meet with generic differences in our domesticated
productions.
When we attempt to estimate the amount
of structural difference between the domestic races
of the same species, we are soon involved in doubt,
from not knowing whether they have descended from one
or several parent-species. This point, if it
could be cleared up, would be interesting; if, for
instance, it could be shown that the greyhound, bloodhound,
terrier, spaniel, and bull-dog, which we all know propagate
their kind so truly, were the offspring of any single
species, then such facts would have great weight in
making us doubt about the immutability of the many
very closely allied and natural species for
instance, of the many foxes inhabiting
different quarters of the world. I do not believe,
as we shall presently see, that all our dogs have descended
from any one wild species; but, in the case of some
other domestic races, there is presumptive, or even
strong, evidence in favour of this view.
It has often been assumed that man
has chosen for domestication animals and plants having
an extraordinary inherent tendency to vary, and likewise
to withstand diverse climates. I do not dispute
that these capacities have added largely to the value
of most of our domesticated productions; but how could
a savage possibly know, when he first tamed an animal,
whether it would vary in succeeding generations, and
whether it would endure other climates? Has the
little variability of the ass or guinea-fowl, or the
small power of endurance of warmth by the rein-deer,
or of cold by the common camel, prevented their domestication?
I cannot doubt that if other animals and plants, equal
in number to our domesticated productions, and belonging
to equally diverse classes and countries, were taken
from a state of nature, and could be made to breed
for an equal number of generations under domestication,
they would vary on an average as largely as the parent
species of our existing domesticated productions have
varied.
In the case of most of our anciently
domesticated animals and plants, I do not think it
is possible to come to any definite conclusion, whether
they have descended from one or several species.
The argument mainly relied on by those who believe
in the multiple origin of our domestic animals is,
that we find in the most ancient records, more especially
on the monuments of Egypt, much diversity in the breeds;
and that some of the breeds closely resemble, perhaps
are identical with, those still existing. Even
if this latter fact were found more strictly and generally
true than seems to me to be the case, what does it
show, but that some of our breeds originated there,
four or five thousand years ago? But Mr. Horner’s
researches have rendered it in some degree probable
that man sufficiently civilized to have manufactured
pottery existed in the valley of the Nile thirteen
or fourteen thousand years ago; and who will pretend
to say how long before these ancient periods, savages,
like those of Tierra del Fuego or Australia,
who possess a semi-domestic dog, may not have existed
in Egypt?
The whole subject must, I think, remain
vague; nevertheless, I may, without here entering
on any details, state that, from geographical and
other considerations, I think it highly probable that
our domestic dogs have descended from several wild
species. In regard to sheep and goats I can form
no opinion. I should think, from facts communicated
to me by Mr. Blyth, on the habits, voice, and constitution,
etc., of the humped Indian cattle, that these
had descended from a different aboriginal stock from
our European cattle; and several competent judges believe
that these latter have had more than one wild parent.
With respect to horses, from reasons which I cannot
give here, I am doubtfully inclined to believe, in
opposition to several authors, that all the races have
descended from one wild stock. Mr. Blyth, whose
opinion, from his large and varied stores of knowledge,
I should value more than that of almost any one, thinks
that all the breeds of poultry have proceeded from
the common wild Indian fowl (Gallus bankiva).
In regard to ducks and rabbits, the breeds of which
differ considerably from each other in structure,
I do not doubt that they all have descended from the
common wild duck and rabbit.
The doctrine of the origin of our
several domestic races from several aboriginal stocks,
has been carried to an absurd extreme by some authors.
They believe that every race which breeds true, let
the distinctive characters be ever so slight, has
had its wild prototype. At this rate there must
have existed at least a score of species of wild cattle,
as many sheep, and several goats in Europe alone, and
several even within Great Britain. One author
believes that there formerly existed in Great Britain
eleven wild species of sheep peculiar to it!
When we bear in mind that Britain has now hardly one
peculiar mammal, and France but few distinct from
those of Germany and conversely, and so with Hungary,
Spain, etc., but that each of these kingdoms possesses
several peculiar breeds of cattle, sheep, etc.,
we must admit that many domestic breeds have originated
in Europe; for whence could they have been derived,
as these several countries do not possess a number
of peculiar species as distinct parent-stocks?
So it is in India. Even in the case of the domestic
dogs of the whole world, which I fully admit have
probably descended from several wild species, I cannot
doubt that there has been an immense amount of inherited
variation. Who can believe that animals closely
resembling the Italian greyhound, the bloodhound,
the bull-dog, or Blenheim spaniel, etc. so
unlike all wild Canidae ever existed freely
in a state of nature? It has often been loosely
said that all our races of dogs have been produced
by the crossing of a few aboriginal species; but by
crossing we can get only forms in some degree intermediate
between their parents; and if we account for our several
domestic races by this process, we must admit the
former existence of the most extreme forms, as the
Italian greyhound, bloodhound, bull-dog, etc.,
in the wild state. Moreover, the possibility
of making distinct races by crossing has been greatly
exaggerated. There can be no doubt that a race
may be modified by occasional crosses, if aided by
the careful selection of those individual mongrels,
which present any desired character; but that a race
could be obtained nearly intermediate between two extremely
different races or species, I can hardly believe.
Sir J. Sebright expressly experimentised for this
object, and failed. The offspring from the first
cross between two pure breeds is tolerably and sometimes
(as I have found with pigeons) extremely uniform,
and everything seems simple enough; but when these
mongrels are crossed one with another for several
generations, hardly two of them will be alike, and
then the extreme difficulty, or rather utter hopelessness,
of the task becomes apparent. Certainly, a breed
intermediate between two very distinct
breeds could not be got without extreme care and long-continued
selection; nor can I find a single case on record
of a permanent race having been thus formed.
On the breeds of
the domestic pigeon: Believing
that it is always best to study some special group,
I have, after deliberation, taken up domestic pigeons.
I have kept every breed which I could purchase or
obtain, and have been most kindly favoured with skins
from several quarters of the world, more especially
by the Honourable W. Elliot from India, and by the
Honourable C. Murray from Persia. Many treatises
in different languages have been published on pigeons,
and some of them are very important, as being of considerable
antiquity. I have associated with several eminent
fanciers, and have been permitted to join two of the
London Pigeon Clubs. The diversity of the breeds
is something astonishing. Compare the English
carrier and the short-faced tumbler, and see the wonderful
difference in their beaks, entailing corresponding
differences in their skulls. The carrier, more
especially the male bird, is also remarkable from the
wonderful development of the carunculated skin about
the head, and this is accompanied by greatly elongated
eyelids, very large external orifices to the nostrils,
and a wide gape of mouth. The short-faced tumbler
has a beak in outline almost like that of a finch;
and the common tumbler has the singular and strictly
inherited habit of flying at a great height in a compact
flock, and tumbling in the air head over heels.
The runt is a bird of great size, with long, massive
beak and large feet; some of the sub-breeds of runts
have very long necks, others very long wings and tails,
others singularly short tails. The barb is allied
to the carrier, but, instead of a very long beak,
has a very short and very broad one. The pouter
has a much elongated body, wings, and legs; and its
enormously developed crop, which it glories in inflating,
may well excite astonishment and even laughter.
The turbit has a very short and conical beak, with
a line of reversed feathers down the breast; and it
has the habit of continually expanding slightly the
upper part of the oesophagus. The Jacobin has
the feathers so much reversed along the back of the
neck that they form a hood, and it has, proportionally
to its size, much elongated wing and tail feathers.
The trumpeter and laugher, as their names express,
utter a very different coo from the other breeds.
The fantail has thirty or even forty tail-feathers,
instead of twelve or fourteen, the normal number in
all members of the great pigeon family; and these
feathers are kept expanded, and are carried so erect
that in good birds the head and tail touch; the oil-gland
is quite aborted. Several other less distinct
breeds might have been specified.
In the skeletons of the several breeds,
the development of the bones of the face in length
and breadth and curvature differs enormously.
The shape, as well as the breadth and length of the
ramus of the lower jaw, varies in a highly remarkable
manner. The number of the caudal and sacral vertebrae
vary; as does the number of the ribs, together with
their relative breadth and the presence of processes.
The size and shape of the apertures in the sternum
are highly variable; so is the degree of divergence
and relative size of the two arms of the furcula.
The proportional width of the gape of mouth, the proportional
length of the eyelids, of the orifice of the nostrils,
of the tongue (not always in strict correlation with
the length of beak), the size of the crop and of the
upper part of the oesophagus; the development and abortion
of the oil-gland; the number of the primary wing and
caudal feathers; the relative length of wing and tail
to each other and to the body; the relative length
of leg and of the feet; the number of scutellae
on the toes, the development of skin between the toes,
are all points of structure which are variable.
The period at which the perfect plumage is acquired
varies, as does the state of the down with which the
nestling birds are clothed when hatched. The
shape and size of the eggs vary. The manner of
flight differs remarkably; as does in some breeds the
voice and disposition. Lastly, in certain breeds,
the males and females have come to differ to a slight
degree from each other.
Altogether at least a score of pigeons
might be chosen, which if shown to an ornithologist,
and he were told that they were wild birds, would
certainly, I think, be ranked by him as well-defined
species. Moreover, I do not believe that any
ornithologist would place the English carrier, the
short-faced tumbler, the runt, the barb, pouter, and
fantail in the same genus; more especially as in each
of these breeds several truly-inherited sub-breeds,
or species as he might have called them, could be
shown him.
Great as the differences are between
the breeds of pigeons, I am fully convinced that the
common opinion of naturalists is correct, namely,
that all have descended from the rock-pigeon (Columba
livia), including under this term several geographical
races or sub-species, which differ from each other
in the most trifling respects. As several of the
reasons which have led me to this belief are in some
degree applicable in other cases, I will here briefly
give them. If the several breeds are not varieties,
and have not proceeded from the rock-pigeon, they must
have descended from at least seven or eight aboriginal
stocks; for it is impossible to make the present domestic
breeds by the crossing of any lesser number:
how, for instance, could a pouter be produced by crossing
two breeds unless one of the parent-stocks possessed
the characteristic enormous crop? The supposed
aboriginal stocks must all have been rock-pigeons,
that is, not breeding or willingly perching on trees.
But besides C. livia, with its geographical sub-species,
only two or three other species of rock-pigeons are
known; and these have not any of the characters of
the domestic breeds. Hence the supposed aboriginal
stocks must either still exist in the countries where
they were originally domesticated, and yet be unknown
to ornithologists; and this, considering their size,
habits, and remarkable characters, seems very improbable;
or they must have become extinct in the wild state.
But birds breeding on precipices, and good fliers,
are unlikely to be exterminated; and the common rock-pigeon,
which has the same habits with the domestic breeds,
has not been exterminated even on several of the smaller
British islets, or on the shores of the Mediterranean.
Hence the supposed extermination of so many species
having similar habits with the rock-pigeon seems to
me a very rash assumption. Moreover, the several
above-named domesticated breeds have been transported
to all parts of the world, and, therefore, some of
them must have been carried back again into their
native country; but not one has ever become wild or
feral, though the dovecot-pigeon, which is the rock-pigeon
in a very slightly altered state, has become feral
in several places. Again, all recent experience
shows that it is most difficult to get any wild animal
to breed freely under domestication; yet on the hypothesis
of the multiple origin of our pigeons, it must be
assumed that at least seven or eight species were
so thoroughly domesticated in ancient times by half-civilized
man, as to be quite prolific under confinement.
An argument, as it seems to me, of
great weight, and applicable in several other cases,
is, that the above-specified breeds, though agreeing
generally in constitution, habits, voice, colouring,
and in most parts of their structure, with the wild
rock-pigeon, yet are certainly highly abnormal in
other parts of their structure: we may look in
vain throughout the whole great family of Columbidae
for a beak like that of the English carrier, or that
of the short-faced tumbler, or barb; for reversed
feathers like those of the jacobin; for a crop like
that of the pouter; for tail-feathers like those of
the fantail. Hence it must be assumed not only
that half-civilized man succeeded in thoroughly domesticating
several species, but that he intentionally or by chance
picked out extraordinarily abnormal species; and further,
that these very species have since all become extinct
or unknown. So many strange contingencies seem
to me improbable in the highest degree.
Some facts in regard to the colouring
of pigeons well deserve consideration. The rock-pigeon
is of a slaty-blue, and has a white rump (the Indian
sub-species, C. intermedia of Strickland, having
it bluish); the tail has a terminal dark bar, with
the bases of the outer feathers externally edged with
white; the wings have two black bars; some semi-domestic
breeds and some apparently truly wild breeds have,
besides the two black bars, the wings chequered with
black. These several marks do not occur together
in any other species of the whole family. Now,
in every one of the domestic breeds, taking thoroughly
well-bred birds, all the above marks, even to the
white edging of the outer tail-feathers, sometimes
concur perfectly developed. Moreover, when two
birds belonging to two distinct breeds are crossed,
neither of which is blue or has any of the above-specified
marks, the mongrel offspring are very apt suddenly
to acquire these characters; for instance, I crossed
some uniformly white fantails with some uniformly
black barbs, and they produced mottled brown and black
birds; these I again crossed together, and one grandchild
of the pure white fantail and pure black barb was of
as beautiful a blue colour, with the white rump, double
black wing-bar, and barred and white-edged tail-feathers,
as any wild rock-pigeon! We can understand these
facts, on the well-known principle of reversion to
ancestral characters, if all the domestic breeds have
descended from the rock-pigeon. But if we deny
this, we must make one of the two following highly
improbable suppositions. Either, firstly, that
all the several imagined aboriginal stocks were coloured
and marked like the rock-pigeon, although no other
existing species is thus coloured and marked, so that
in each separate breed there might be a tendency to
revert to the very same colours and markings.
Or, secondly, that each breed, even the purest, has
within a dozen or, at most, within a score of generations,
been crossed by the rock-pigeon: I say within
a dozen or twenty generations, for we know of no fact
countenancing the belief that the child ever reverts
to some one ancestor, removed by a greater number
of generations. In a breed which has been crossed
only once with some distinct breed, the tendency to
reversion to any character derived from such cross
will naturally become less and less, as in each succeeding
generation there will be less of the foreign blood;
but when there has been no cross with a distinct breed,
and there is a tendency in both parents to revert
to a character, which has been lost during some former
generation, this tendency, for all that we can see
to the contrary, may be transmitted undiminished for
an indefinite number of generations. These two
distinct cases are often confounded in treatises on
inheritance.
Lastly, the hybrids or mongrels from
between all the domestic breeds of pigeons are perfectly
fertile. I can state this from my own observations,
purposely made on the most distinct breeds. Now,
it is difficult, perhaps impossible, to bring forward
one case of the hybrid offspring of two animals clearly
distinct being themselves perfectly fertile.
Some authors believe that long-continued domestication
eliminates this strong tendency to sterility:
from the history of the dog I think there is some
probability in this hypothesis, if applied to species
closely related together, though it is unsupported
by a single experiment. But to extend the hypothesis
so far as to suppose that species, aboriginally as
distinct as carriers, tumblers, pouters, and fantails
now are, should yield offspring perfectly fertile,
inter se, seems to me rash in the extreme.
From these several reasons, namely,
the improbability of man having formerly got seven
or eight supposed species of pigeons to breed freely
under domestication; these supposed species being quite
unknown in a wild state, and their becoming nowhere
feral; these species having very abnormal characters
in certain respects, as compared with all other Columbidae,
though so like in most other respects to the rock-pigeon;
the blue colour and various marks occasionally appearing
in all the breeds, both when kept pure and when crossed;
the mongrel offspring being perfectly fertile; from
these several reasons, taken together, I can feel
no doubt that all our domestic breeds have descended
from the Columba livia with its geographical sub-species.
In favour of this view, I may add,
firstly, that C. livia, or the rock-pigeon, has been
found capable of domestication in Europe and in India;
and that it agrees in habits and in a great number
of points of structure with all the domestic breeds.
Secondly, although an English carrier or short-faced
tumbler differs immensely in certain characters from
the rock-pigeon, yet by comparing the several sub-breeds
of these breeds, more especially those brought from
distant countries, we can make an almost perfect series
between the extremes of structure. Thirdly, those
characters which are mainly distinctive of each breed,
for instance the wattle and length of beak of the carrier,
the shortness of that of the tumbler, and the number
of tail-feathers in the fantail, are in each breed
eminently variable; and the explanation of this fact
will be obvious when we come to treat of selection.
Fourthly, pigeons have been watched, and tended with
the utmost care, and loved by many people. They
have been domesticated for thousands of years in several
quarters of the world; the earliest known record of
pigeons is in the fifth Aegyptian dynasty, about 3000
B.C., as was pointed out to me by Professor Lepsius;
but Mr. Birch informs me that pigeons are given in
a bill of fare in the previous dynasty. In the
time of the Romans, as we hear from Pliny, immense
prices were given for pigeons; “nay, they are
come to this pass, that they can reckon up their pedigree
and race.” Pigeons were much valued by
Akber Khan in India, about the year 1600; never less
than 20,000 pigeons were taken with the court.
“The monarchs of Iran and Turan sent him some
very rare birds;” and, continues the courtly
historian, “His Majesty by crossing the breeds,
which method was never practised before, has improved
them astonishingly.” About this same period
the Dutch were as eager about pigeons as were the old
Romans. The paramount importance of these considerations
in explaining the immense amount of variation which
pigeons have undergone, will be obvious when we treat
of Selection. We shall then, also, see how it
is that the breeds so often have a somewhat monstrous
character. It is also a most favourable circumstance
for the production of distinct breeds, that male and
female pigeons can be easily mated for life; and thus
different breeds can be kept together in the same aviary.
I have discussed the probable origin
of domestic pigeons at some, yet quite insufficient,
length; because when I first kept pigeons and watched
the several kinds, knowing well how true they bred,
I felt fully as much difficulty in believing that
they could ever have descended from a common parent,
as any naturalist could in coming to a similar conclusion
in regard to the many species of finches, or other
large groups of birds, in nature. One circumstance
has struck me much; namely, that all the breeders
of the various domestic animals and the cultivators
of plants, with whom I have ever conversed, or whose
treatises I have read, are firmly convinced that the
several breeds to which each has attended, are descended
from so many aboriginally distinct species. Ask,
as I have asked, a celebrated raiser of Hereford cattle,
whether his cattle might not have descended from long
horns, and he will laugh you to scorn. I have
never met a pigeon, or poultry, or duck, or rabbit
fancier, who was not fully convinced that each main
breed was descended from a distinct species. Van
Mons, in his treatise on pears and apples, shows how
utterly he disbelieves that the several sorts, for
instance a Ribston-pippin or Codlin-apple, could ever
have proceeded from the seeds of the same tree.
Innumerable other examples could be given. The
explanation, I think, is simple: from long-continued
study they are strongly impressed with the differences
between the several races; and though they well know
that each race varies slightly, for they win their
prizes by selecting such slight differences, yet they
ignore all general arguments, and refuse to sum up
in their minds slight differences accumulated during
many successive generations. May not those naturalists
who, knowing far less of the laws of inheritance than
does the breeder, and knowing no more than he does
of the intermediate links in the long lines of descent,
yet admit that many of our domestic races have descended
from the same parents may they not learn
a lesson of caution, when they deride the idea of
species in a state of nature being lineal descendants
of other species?
Selection. Let us now
briefly consider the steps by which domestic races
have been produced, either from one or from several
allied species. Some little effect may, perhaps,
be attributed to the direct action of the external
conditions of life, and some little to habit; but he
would be a bold man who would account by such agencies
for the differences of a dray and race horse, a greyhound
and bloodhound, a carrier and tumbler pigeon.
One of the most remarkable features in our domesticated
races is that we see in them adaptation, not indeed
to the animal’s or plant’s own good, but
to man’s use or fancy. Some variations useful
to him have probably arisen suddenly, or by one step;
many botanists, for instance, believe that the fuller’s
teazle, with its hooks, which cannot be rivalled by
any mechanical contrivance, is only a variety of the
wild Dipsacus; and this amount of change may have
suddenly arisen in a seedling. So it has probably
been with the turnspit dog; and this is known to have
been the case with the ancon sheep. But when
we compare the dray-horse and race-horse, the dromedary
and camel, the various breeds of sheep fitted either
for cultivated land or mountain pasture, with the wool
of one breed good for one purpose, and that of another
breed for another purpose; when we compare the many
breeds of dogs, each good for man in very different
ways; when we compare the game-cock, so pertinacious
in battle, with other breeds so little quarrelsome,
with “everlasting layers” which never
desire to sit, and with the bantam so small and elegant;
when we compare the host of agricultural, culinary,
orchard, and flower-garden races of plants, most useful
to man at different seasons and for different purposes,
or so beautiful in his eyes, we must, I think, look
further than to mere variability. We cannot suppose
that all the breeds were suddenly produced as perfect
and as useful as we now see them; indeed, in several
cases, we know that this has not been their history.
The key is man’s power of accumulative selection:
nature gives successive variations; man adds them up
in certain directions useful to him. In this
sense he may be said to make for himself useful breeds.
The great power of this principle
of selection is not hypothetical. It is certain
that several of our eminent breeders have, even within
a single lifetime, modified to a large extent some
breeds of cattle and sheep. In order fully to
realise what they have done, it is almost necessary
to read several of the many treatises devoted to this
subject, and to inspect the animals. Breeders
habitually speak of an animal’s organisation
as something quite plastic, which they can model almost
as they please. If I had space I could quote numerous
passages to this effect from highly competent authorities.
Youatt, who was probably better acquainted with the
works of agriculturalists than almost any other individual,
and who was himself a very good judge of an animal,
speaks of the principle of selection as “that
which enables the agriculturist, not only to modify
the character of his flock, but to change it altogether.
It is the magician’s wand, by means of which
he may summon into life whatever form and mould he
pleases.” Lord Somerville, speaking of
what breeders have done for sheep, says: “It
would seem as if they had chalked out upon a wall a
form perfect in itself, and then had given it existence.”
That most skilful breeder, Sir John Sebright, used
to say, with respect to pigeons, that “he would
produce any given feather in three years, but it would
take him six years to obtain head and beak.”
In Saxony the importance of the principle of selection
in regard to merino sheep is so fully recognised,
that men follow it as a trade: the sheep are placed
on a table and are studied, like a picture by a connoisseur;
this is done three times at intervals of months, and
the sheep are each time marked and classed, so that
the very best may ultimately be selected for breeding.
What English breeders have actually
effected is proved by the enormous prices given for
animals with a good pedigree; and these have now been
exported to almost every quarter of the world.
The improvement is by no means generally due to crossing
different breeds; all the best breeders are strongly
opposed to this practice, except sometimes amongst
closely allied sub-breeds. And when a cross has
been made, the closest selection is far more indispensable
even than in ordinary cases. If selection consisted
merely in separating some very distinct variety, and
breeding from it, the principle would be so obvious
as hardly to be worth notice; but its importance consists
in the great effect produced by the accumulation in
one direction, during successive generations, of differences
absolutely inappreciable by an uneducated eye differences
which I for one have vainly attempted to appreciate.
Not one man in a thousand has accuracy of eye and
judgment sufficient to become an eminent breeder.
If gifted with these qualities, and he studies his
subject for years, and devotes his lifetime to it with
indomitable perseverance, he will succeed, and may
make great improvements; if he wants any of these
qualities, he will assuredly fail. Few would readily
believe in the natural capacity and years of practice
requisite to become even a skilful pigeon-fancier.
The same principles are followed by
horticulturists; but the variations are here often
more abrupt. No one supposes that our choicest
productions have been produced by a single variation
from the aboriginal stock. We have proofs that
this is not so in some cases, in which exact records
have been kept; thus, to give a very trifling instance,
the steadily-increasing size of the common gooseberry
may be quoted. We see an astonishing improvement
in many florists’ flowers, when the flowers
of the present day are compared with drawings made
only twenty or thirty years ago. When a race
of plants is once pretty well established, the seed-raisers
do not pick out the best plants, but merely go over
their seed-beds, and pull up the “rogues,”
as they call the plants that deviate from the proper
standard. With animals this kind of selection
is, in fact, also followed; for hardly any one is so
careless as to allow his worst animals to breed.
In regard to plants, there is another
means of observing the accumulated effects of selection namely,
by comparing the diversity of flowers in the different
varieties of the same species in the flower-garden;
the diversity of leaves, pods, or tubers, or whatever
part is valued, in the kitchen-garden, in comparison
with the flowers of the same varieties; and the diversity
of fruit of the same species in the orchard, in comparison
with the leaves and flowers of the same set of varieties.
See how different the leaves of the cabbage are, and
how extremely alike the flowers; how unlike the flowers
of the heartsease are, and how alike the leaves; how
much the fruit of the different kinds of gooseberries
differ in size, colour, shape, and hairiness, and
yet the flowers present very slight differences.
It is not that the varieties which differ largely
in some one point do not differ at all in other points;
this is hardly ever, perhaps never, the case.
The laws of correlation of growth, the importance
of which should never be overlooked, will ensure some
differences; but, as a general rule, I cannot doubt
that the continued selection of slight variations,
either in the leaves, the flowers, or the fruit, will
produce races differing from each other chiefly in
these characters.
It may be objected that the principle
of selection has been reduced to methodical practice
for scarcely more than three-quarters of a century;
it has certainly been more attended to of late years,
and many treatises have been published on the subject;
and the result, I may add, has been, in a corresponding
degree, rapid and important. But it is very far
from true that the principle is a modern discovery.
I could give several references to the full acknowledgment
of the importance of the principle in works of high
antiquity. In rude and barbarous periods of English
history choice animals were often imported, and laws
were passed to prevent their exportation: the
destruction of horses under a certain size was ordered,
and this may be compared to the “roguing”
of plants by nurserymen. The principle of selection
I find distinctly given in an ancient Chinese encyclopaedia.
Explicit rules are laid down by some of the Roman
classical writers. From passages in Genesis, it
is clear that the colour of domestic animals was at
that early period attended to. Savages now sometimes
cross their dogs with wild canine animals, to improve
the breed, and they formerly did so, as is attested
by passages in Pliny. The savages in South Africa
match their draught cattle by colour, as do some of
the Esquimaux their teams of dogs. Livingstone
shows how much good domestic breeds are valued by the
negroes of the interior of Africa who have not associated
with Europeans. Some of these facts do not show
actual selection, but they show that the breeding of
domestic animals was carefully attended to in ancient
times, and is now attended to by the lowest savages.
It would, indeed, have been a strange fact, had attention
not been paid to breeding, for the inheritance of
good and bad qualities is so obvious.
At the present time, eminent breeders
try by methodical selection, with a distinct object
in view, to make a new strain or sub-breed, superior
to anything existing in the country. But, for
our purpose, a kind of Selection, which may be called
Unconscious, and which results from every one trying
to possess and breed from the best individual animals,
is more important. Thus, a man who intends keeping
pointers naturally tries to get as good dogs as he
can, and afterwards breeds from his own best dogs,
but he has no wish or expectation of permanently altering
the breed. Nevertheless I cannot doubt that this
process, continued during centuries, would improve
and modify any breed, in the same way as Bakewell,
Collins, etc., by this very same process, only
carried on more methodically, did greatly modify,
even during their own lifetimes, the forms and qualities
of their cattle. Slow and insensible changes of
this kind could never be recognised unless actual
measurements or careful drawings of the breeds in
question had been made long ago, which might serve
for comparison. In some cases, however, unchanged
or but little changed individuals of the same breed
may be found in less civilised districts, where the
breed has been less improved. There is reason
to believe that King Charles’s spaniel has been
unconsciously modified to a large extent since the
time of that monarch. Some highly competent authorities
are convinced that the setter is directly derived from
the spaniel, and has probably been slowly altered
from it. It is known that the English pointer
has been greatly changed within the last century,
and in this case the change has, it is believed, been
chiefly effected by crosses with the fox-hound; but
what concerns us is, that the change has been effected
unconsciously and gradually, and yet so effectually,
that, though the old Spanish pointer certainly came
from Spain, Mr. Borrow has not seen, as I am informed
by him, any native dog in Spain like our pointer.
By a similar process of selection,
and by careful training, the whole body of English
racehorses have come to surpass in fleetness and size
the parent Arab stock, so that the latter, by the regulations
for the Goodwood Races, are favoured in the weights
they carry. Lord Spencer and others have shown
how the cattle of England have increased in weight
and in early maturity, compared with the stock formerly
kept in this country. By comparing the accounts
given in old pigeon treatises of carriers and tumblers
with these breeds as now existing in Britain, India,
and Persia, we can, I think, clearly trace the stages
through which they have insensibly passed, and come
to differ so greatly from the rock-pigeon.
Youatt gives an excellent illustration
of the effects of a course of selection, which may
be considered as unconsciously followed, in so far
that the breeders could never have expected or even
have wished to have produced the result which ensued namely,
the production of two distinct strains. The two
flocks of Leicester sheep kept by Mr. Buckley and Mr.
Burgess, as Mr. Youatt remarks, “have been purely
bred from the original stock of Mr. Bakewell for upwards
of fifty years. There is not a suspicion existing
in the mind of any one at all acquainted with the
subject that the owner of either of them has deviated
in any one instance from the pure blood of Mr. Bakewell’s
flock, and yet the difference between the sheep possessed
by these two gentlemen is so great that they have
the appearance of being quite different varieties.”
If there exist savages so barbarous
as never to think of the inherited character of the
offspring of their domestic animals, yet any one animal
particularly useful to them, for any special purpose,
would be carefully preserved during famines and other
accidents, to which savages are so liable, and such
choice animals would thus generally leave more offspring
than the inferior ones; so that in this case there
would be a kind of unconscious selection going on.
We see the value set on animals even by the barbarians
of Tierra del Fuego, by their killing
and devouring their old women, in times of dearth,
as of less value than their dogs.
In plants the same gradual process
of improvement, through the occasional preservation
of the best individuals, whether or not sufficiently
distinct to be ranked at their first appearance as
distinct varieties, and whether or not two or more
species or races have become blended together by crossing,
may plainly be recognised in the increased size and
beauty which we now see in the varieties of the heartsease,
rose, pelargonium, dahlia, and other plants, when compared
with the older varieties or with their parent-stocks.
No one would ever expect to get a first-rate heartsease
or dahlia from the seed of a wild plant. No one
would expect to raise a first-rate melting pear from
the seed of a wild pear, though he might succeed from
a poor seedling growing wild, if it had come from
a garden-stock. The pear, though cultivated in
classical times, appears, from Pliny’s description,
to have been a fruit of very inferior quality.
I have seen great surprise expressed in horticultural
works at the wonderful skill of gardeners, in having
produced such splendid results from such poor materials;
but the art, I cannot doubt, has been simple, and,
as far as the final result is concerned, has been
followed almost unconsciously. It has consisted
in always cultivating the best known variety, sowing
its seeds, and, when a slightly better variety has
chanced to appear, selecting it, and so onwards.
But the gardeners of the classical period, who cultivated
the best pear they could procure, never thought what
splendid fruit we should eat; though we owe our excellent
fruit, in some small degree, to their having naturally
chosen and preserved the best varieties they could
anywhere find.
A large amount of change in our cultivated
plants, thus slowly and unconsciously accumulated,
explains, as I believe, the well-known fact, that
in a vast number of cases we cannot recognise, and
therefore do not know, the wild parent-stocks of the
plants which have been longest cultivated in our flower
and kitchen gardens. If it has taken centuries
or thousands of years to improve or modify most of
our plants up to their present standard of usefulness
to man, we can understand how it is that neither Australia,
the Cape of Good Hope, nor any other region inhabited
by quite uncivilised man, has afforded us a single
plant worth culture. It is not that these countries,
so rich in species, do not by a strange chance possess
the aboriginal stocks of any useful plants, but that
the native plants have not been improved by continued
selection up to a standard of perfection comparable
with that given to the plants in countries anciently
civilised.
In regard to the domestic animals
kept by uncivilised man, it should not be overlooked
that they almost always have to struggle for their
own food, at least during certain seasons. And
in two countries very differently circumstanced, individuals
of the same species, having slightly different constitutions
or structure, would often succeed better in the one
country than in the other, and thus by a process of
“natural selection,” as will hereafter
be more fully explained, two sub-breeds might be formed.
This, perhaps, partly explains what has been remarked
by some authors, namely, that the varieties kept by
savages have more of the character of species than
the varieties kept in civilised countries.
On the view here given of the all-important
part which selection by man has played, it becomes
at once obvious, how it is that our domestic races
show adaptation in their structure or in their habits
to man’s wants or fancies. We can, I think,
further understand the frequently abnormal character
of our domestic races, and likewise their differences
being so great in external characters and relatively
so slight in internal parts or organs. Man can
hardly select, or only with much difficulty, any deviation
of structure excepting such as is externally visible;
and indeed he rarely cares for what is internal.
He can never act by selection, excepting on variations
which are first given to him in some slight degree
by nature. No man would ever try to make a fantail,
till he saw a pigeon with a tail developed in some
slight degree in an unusual manner, or a pouter till
he saw a pigeon with a crop of somewhat unusual size;
and the more abnormal or unusual any character was
when it first appeared, the more likely it would be
to catch his attention. But to use such an expression
as trying to make a fantail, is, I have no doubt,
in most cases, utterly incorrect. The man who
first selected a pigeon with a slightly larger tail,
never dreamed what the descendants of that pigeon
would become through long-continued, partly unconscious
and partly methodical selection. Perhaps the parent
bird of all fantails had only fourteen tail-feathers
somewhat expanded, like the present Java fantail,
or like individuals of other and distinct breeds,
in which as many as seventeen tail-feathers have been
counted. Perhaps the first pouter-pigeon did
not inflate its crop much more than the turbit now
does the upper part of its oesophagus, a
habit which is disregarded by all fanciers, as it
is not one of the points of the breed.
Nor let it be thought that some great
deviation of structure would be necessary to catch
the fancier’s eye: he perceives extremely
small differences, and it is in human nature to value
any novelty, however slight, in one’s own possession.
Nor must the value which would formerly be set on
any slight differences in the individuals of the same
species, be judged of by the value which would now
be set on them, after several breeds have once fairly
been established. Many slight differences might,
and indeed do now, arise amongst pigeons, which are
rejected as faults or deviations from the standard
of perfection of each breed. The common goose
has not given rise to any marked varieties; hence the
Thoulouse and the common breed, which differ only
in colour, that most fleeting of characters, have
lately been exhibited as distinct at our poultry-shows.
I think these views further explain
what has sometimes been noticed namely
that we know nothing about the origin or history of
any of our domestic breeds. But, in fact, a breed,
like a dialect of a language, can hardly be said to
have had a definite origin. A man preserves and
breeds from an individual with some slight deviation
of structure, or takes more care than usual in matching
his best animals and thus improves them, and the improved
individuals slowly spread in the immediate neighbourhood.
But as yet they will hardly have a distinct name,
and from being only slightly valued, their history
will be disregarded. When further improved by
the same slow and gradual process, they will spread
more widely, and will get recognised as something
distinct and valuable, and will then probably first
receive a provincial name. In semi-civilised
countries, with little free communication, the spreading
and knowledge of any new sub-breed will be a slow process.
As soon as the points of value of the new sub-breed
are once fully acknowledged, the principle, as I have
called it, of unconscious selection will always tend, perhaps
more at one period than at another, as the breed rises
or falls in fashion, perhaps more in one
district than in another, according to the state of
civilisation of the inhabitants slowly
to add to the characteristic features of the breed,
whatever they may be. But the chance will be infinitely
small of any record having been preserved of such
slow, varying, and insensible changes.
I must now say a few words on the
circumstances, favourable, or the reverse, to man’s
power of selection. A high degree of variability
is obviously favourable, as freely giving the materials
for selection to work on; not that mere individual
differences are not amply sufficient, with extreme
care, to allow of the accumulation of a large amount
of modification in almost any desired direction.
But as variations manifestly useful or pleasing to
man appear only occasionally, the chance of their
appearance will be much increased by a large number
of individuals being kept; and hence this comes to
be of the highest importance to success. On this
principle Marshall has remarked, with respect to the
sheep of parts of Yorkshire, that “as they generally
belong to poor people, and are mostly in small
lots, they never can be improved.”
On the other hand, nurserymen, from raising large stocks
of the same plants, are generally far more successful
than amateurs in getting new and valuable varieties.
The keeping of a large number of individuals of a
species in any country requires that the species should
be placed under favourable conditions of life, so as
to breed freely in that country. When the individuals
of any species are scanty, all the individuals, whatever
their quality may be, will generally be allowed to
breed, and this will effectually prevent selection.
But probably the most important point of all, is,
that the animal or plant should be so highly useful
to man, or so much valued by him, that the closest
attention should be paid to even the slightest deviation
in the qualities or structure of each individual.
Unless such attention be paid nothing can be effected.
I have seen it gravely remarked, that it was most
fortunate that the strawberry began to vary just when
gardeners began to attend closely to this plant.
No doubt the strawberry had always varied since it
was cultivated, but the slight varieties had been
neglected. As soon, however, as gardeners picked
out individual plants with slightly larger, earlier,
or better fruit, and raised seedlings from them, and
again picked out the best seedlings and bred from them,
then, there appeared (aided by some crossing with distinct
species) those many admirable varieties of the strawberry
which have been raised during the last thirty or forty
years.
In the case of animals with separate
sexes, facility in preventing crosses is an important
element of success in the formation of new races, at
least, in a country which is already stocked with other
races. In this respect enclosure of the land plays
a part. Wandering savages or the inhabitants
of open plains rarely possess more than one breed
of the same species. Pigeons can be mated for
life, and this is a great convenience to the fancier,
for thus many races may be kept true, though mingled
in the same aviary; and this circumstance must have
largely favoured the improvement and formation of new
breeds. Pigeons, I may add, can be propagated
in great numbers and at a very quick rate, and inferior
birds may be freely rejected, as when killed they serve
for food. On the other hand, cats, from their
nocturnal rambling habits, cannot be matched, and,
although so much valued by women and children, we
hardly ever see a distinct breed kept up; such breeds
as we do sometimes see are almost always imported
from some other country, often from islands.
Although I do not doubt that some domestic animals
vary less than others, yet the rarity or absence of
distinct breeds of the cat, the donkey, peacock, goose,
etc., may be attributed in main part to selection
not having been brought into play: in cats, from
the difficulty in pairing them; in donkeys, from only
a few being kept by poor people, and little attention
paid to their breeding; in peacocks, from not being
very easily reared and a large stock not kept; in geese,
from being valuable only for two purposes, food and
feathers, and more especially from no pleasure having
been felt in the display of distinct breeds.
To sum up on the origin of our Domestic
Races of animals and plants. I believe that the
conditions of life, from their action on the reproductive
system, are so far of the highest importance as causing
variability. I do not believe that variability
is an inherent and necessary contingency, under all
circumstances, with all organic beings, as some authors
have thought. The effects of variability are modified
by various degrees of inheritance and of reversion.
Variability is governed by many unknown laws, more
especially by that of correlation of growth.
Something may be attributed to the direct action of
the conditions of life. Something must be attributed
to use and disuse. The final result is thus rendered
infinitely complex. In some cases, I do not doubt
that the intercrossing of species, aboriginally distinct,
has played an important part in the origin of our
domestic productions. When in any country several
domestic breeds have once been established, their
occasional intercrossing, with the aid of selection,
has, no doubt, largely aided in the formation of new
sub-breeds; but the importance of the crossing of
varieties has, I believe, been greatly exaggerated,
both in regard to animals and to those plants which
are propagated by seed. In plants which are temporarily
propagated by cuttings, buds, etc., the importance
of the crossing both of distinct species and of varieties
is immense; for the cultivator here quite disregards
the extreme variability both of hybrids and mongrels,
and the frequent sterility of hybrids; but the cases
of plants not propagated by seed are of little importance
to us, for their endurance is only temporary.
Over all these causes of Change I am convinced that
the accumulative action of Selection, whether applied
methodically and more quickly, or unconsciously and
more slowly, but more efficiently, is by far the predominant
Power.