Parents are anxious that children
should be conversant with Mechanics, and with what
are called the Mechanic Powers. Certainly no species
of knowledge is better suited to the taste and capacity
of youth, and yet it seldom forms a part of early
instruction. Every body talks of the lever, the
wedge, and the pulley, but most people perceive, that
the notions which they have of their respective uses,
are unsatisfactory, and indistinct; and many endeavour,
at a late period of life, to acquire a scientific
and exact knowledge of the effects that are produced
by implements which are in every body’s hands,
or that are absolutely necessary in the daily occupations
of mankind.
An itinerant lecturer seldom fails
of having a numerous and attentive auditory; and if
he does not communicate much of that knowledge which
he endeavours to explain, it is not to be attributed
either to his want of skill, or to the insufficiency
of his apparatus, but to the novelty of the terms
which he is obliged to use. Ignorance of the
language in which any science is taught, is an insuperable
bar to its being suddenly acquired; besides a precise
knowledge of the meaning of terms, we must have an
instantaneous idea excited in our minds whenever they
are repeated; and, as this can be acquired only by
practice, it is impossible that philosophical lectures
can be of much service to those who are not familiarly
acquainted with the technical language in which they
are delivered; and yet there is scarcely any subject
of human inquiry more obvious to the understanding,
than the laws of mechanics. Only a small portion
of geometry is necessary to the learner, if he even
wishes to become master of the more difficult problems
which are usually contained in a course of lectures,
and most of what is practically useful, may be acquired
by any person who is expert in common arithmetic.
But we cannot proceed a single step
without deviating from common language; if the theory
of the balance, or the lever, is to be explained,
we immediately speak of space and time.
To persons not versed in literature, it is probable
that these terms appear more simple and unintelligible
than they do to a man who has read Locke, and other
metaphysical writers. The term space to
the bulk of mankind, conveys the idea of an interval;
they consider the word time as representing
a definite number of years, days, or minutes; but
the metaphysician, when he hears the words space
and time, immediately takes the alarm, and
recurs to the abstract notions which are associated
with these terms; he perceives difficulties unknown
to the unlearned, and feels a confusion of ideas which
distracts his attention. The lecturer proceeds
with confidence, never supposing that his audience
can be puzzled by such common terms. He means
by space, the distance from the place whence
a body begins to fall, to the place where its motion
ceases; and by time, he means the number of seconds,
or of any determinate divisions of civil time
which elapse from the commencement of any motion to
its end; or, in other words, the duration of any given
motion. After this has been frequently repeated,
any intelligent person perceives the sense in which
they are used by the tenour of the discourse; but
in the interim, the greatest part of what he has heard,
cannot have been understood, and the premises upon
which every subsequent demonstration is founded, are
unknown to him. If this be true, when it is affirmed
of two terms only, what must be the situation of those
to whom eight or ten unknown technical terms occur
at the commencement of a lecture? A complete
knowledge, such a knowledge as is not only full, but
familiar, of all the common terms made use of in theoretic
and practical mechanics, is, therefore, absolutely
necessary before any person can attend public lectures
in natural philosophy with advantage.
What has been said of public lectures,
may, with equal propriety, be applied to private instruction;
and it is probable, that inattention to this circumstance
is the reason why so few people have distinct notions
of natural philosophy. Learning by rote, or even
reading repeatedly, definitions of the technical terms
of any science, must undoubtedly facilitate its acquirement;
but conversation, with the habit of explaining the
meaning of words, and the structure of common domestic
implements, to children, is the sure and effectual
method of preparing the mind for the acquirement of
science.
The ancients, in learning this species
of knowledge, had an advantage of which we are deprived:
many of their terms of science were the common names
of familiar objects. How few do we meet who have
a distinct notion of the words radius, angle, or valve.
A Roman peasant knew what a radius or a valve meant,
in their original signification, as well as a modern
professor; he knew that a valve was a door, and a
radius a spoke of a wheel; but an English child finds
it as difficult to remember the meaning of the word
angle, as the word parabola. An angle is usually
confounded, by those who are ignorant of geometry and
mechanics, with the word triangle, and the long reasoning
of many a laborious instructer has been confounded
by this popular mistake. When a glass pump is
shown to an admiring spectator, he is desired to watch
the motion of the valves: he looks “above,
about, and underneath;” but, ignorant of the
word valve, he looks in vain. Had he been
desired to look at the motion of the little doors that
opened and shut, as the handle of the pump was moved
up and down, he would have followed the lecturer with
ease, and would have understood all his subsequent
reasoning. If a child attempts to push any thing
heavier than himself, his feet slide away from it,
and the object can be moved only at intervals, and
by sudden starts; but if he be desired to prop his
feet against the wall, he finds it easy to push what
before eluded his little strength. Here the use
of a fulcrum, or fixed point, by means of which bodies
may be moved, is distinctly understood. If two
boys lay a board across a narrow block of wood, or
stone, and balance each other at the opposite ends
of it, they acquire another idea of a centre of motion.
If a poker is rested against a bar of a grate, and
employed to lift up the coals, the same notion of a
centre is recalled to their minds. If a boy,
sitting upon a plank, a sofa, or form, be lifted up
by another boy’s applying his strength at one
end of the seat, whilst the other end of the seat
rests on the ground, it will be readily perceived
by them, that the point of rest, or centre of motion,
or fulcrum, is the ground, and that the fulcrum is
not, as in the first instance, between the force that
lifts, and the thing that is lifted; the fulcrum is
at one end, the force which is exerted acts at the
other end, and the weight is in the middle. In
trying, these simple experiments, the terms fulcrum,
centre of motion, &c. should be constantly
employed, and in a very short time they would be as
familiar to a boy of eight years old as to any philosopher.
If for some years the same words frequently recur
to him in the same sense, is it to be supposed that
a lecture upon the balance and the lever would be
as unintelligible to him as to persons of good abilities,
who at a more advanced age hear these terms from the
mouth of a lecturer? A boy in such circumstances
would appear as if he had a genius for mechanics,
when, perhaps, he might have less taste for the science,
and less capacity, than the generality of the audience.
Trifling as it may at first appear, it will not be
found a trifling advantage, in the progress of education,
to attend to this circumstance. A distinct knowledge
of a few terms, assists a learner in his first attempts;
finding these successful, he advances with confidence,
and acquires new ideas without difficulty or disgust.
Rousseau, with his usual eloquence, has inculcated
the necessity of annexing ideas to words; he declaims
against the splendid ignorance of men who speak by
rote, and who are rich in words amidst the most deplorable
poverty of ideas. To store the memory of his
pupil with images of things, he is willing to neglect,
and leave to hazard, his acquirement of language.
It requires no elaborate argument to prove that a
boy, whose mind was stored with accurate images of
external objects, of experimental knowledge, and who
had acquired habitual dexterity, but who was unacquainted
with the usual signs by which ideas are expressed,
would be incapable of accurate reasoning, or would,
at best, reason only upon particulars. Without
general terms, he could not abstract; he could not,
until his vocabulary was enlarged, and familiar to
him, reason upon general topics, or draw conclusions
from general principles: in short, he would be
in the situation of those who, in the solution of difficult
and complicated questions relative to quantity, are
obliged to employ tedious and perplexed calculations,
instead of the clear and comprehensive methods that
unfold themselves by the use of signs in algebra.
It is not necessary, in teaching children
the technical language of any art or science, that
we should pursue the same order that is requisite
in teaching the science itself. Order is required
in reasoning, because all reasoning is employed in
deducing propositions from one another in a regular
series; but where terms are employed merely as names,
this order may be dispensed with. It is, however,
of great consequence to seize the proper time for
introducing a new term; a moment when attention is
awake, and when accident has produced some particular
interest in the object. In every family, opportunities
of this sort occur without any preparation, and such
opportunities are far preferable to a formal lecture
and a splendid apparatus for the first lessons in
natural philosophy and chemistry. If the pump
belonging to the house is out of order, and the pump-maker
is set to work, an excellent opportunity presents
itself for variety of instruction. The centre
pin of the handle is taken out, and a long rod is
drawn up by degrees, at the end of which a round piece
of wood is seen partly covered with leather.
Your pupil immediately asks the name of it, and the
pump-maker prevents your answer, by informing little
master that it is called a sucker. You show it
to the child, he handles it, feels whether the leather
is hard or soft, and at length discovers that there
is a hole through it which is covered with a little
flap or door. This, he learns from the workmen,
is called a clack. The child should now be permitted
to plunge the piston (by which name it should
now be called) into a tub of water; in drawing
it backwards and forwards, he will perceive that the
clack, which should now be called the valve, opens
and shuts as the piston is drawn backwards and forwards.
It will be better not to inform the child how this
mechanism is employed in the pump. If the names
sucker and piston, clack and valve, are fixed in his
memory, it will be sufficient for his first lesson.
At another opportunity, he should be present when
the fixed or lower valve of the pump is drawn up; he
will examine it, and find that it is similar to the
valve of the piston; if he sees it put down into the
pump, and sees the piston put into its place, and
set to work, the names that he has learned will be
fixed more deeply in his mind, and he will have some
general notion of the whole apparatus. From time
to time these names should be recalled to his memory
on suitable occasions, but he should not be asked to
repeat them by rote. What has been said, is not
intended as a lesson for a child in mechanics, but
as a sketch of a method of teaching which has been
employed with success.
Whatever repairs are carried on in
a house, children should be permitted to see:
whilst every body about them seems interested, they
become attentive from sympathy; and whenever action
accompanies instruction, it is sure to make an impression.
If a lock is out of order, when it is taken off, show
it to your pupil; point out some of its principal
parts, and name them; then put it into the hands of
a child, and let him manage it as he pleases.
Locks are full of oil, and black with dust and iron;
but if children have been taught habits of neatness,
they may be clock-makers and white-smiths, without
spoiling their clothes, or the furniture of a house.
Upon every occasion of this sort, technical terms
should be made familiar; they are of great use in
the every-day business of life, and are peculiarly
serviceable in giving orders to workmen, who, when
they are spoken to in a language that they are used
to, comprehend what is said to them, and work with
alacrity.
An early use of a rule and pencil,
and easy access to prints of machines, of architecture,
and of the implements of trades, are of obvious use
in this part of education. The machines published
by the Society of Arts in London; the prints in Desaguliers,
Emerson, lé Spectacle de la Nature, Machines
approuvees par l’Academie, Chambers’s
Dictionary, Berthoud sur l’Horlogerie, Dictionaire
des Arts et des Metiers,
may, in succession, be put into the hands of children.
The most simple should be first selected, and the
pupils should be accustomed to attend minutely to
one print before another is given to them. A
proper person should carefully point out and explain
to them the first prints that they examine; they may
afterwards be left to themselves.
To understand prints of machines,
a previous knowledge of what is meant by an elevation,
a profile, a section, a perspective view, and a (vue
d’oiseau) bird’s eye view, is necessary.
To obtain distinct ideas of sections, a few models
of common furniture, as chests of drawers, bellows,
grates, &c. may be provided, and may be cut asunder
in different directions. Children easily comprehend
this part of drawing, and its uses, which may be pointed
out in books of architecture; its application to the
common business of life, is so various and immediate,
as to fix it for ever in the memory; besides, the habit
of abstraction, which is acquired by drawing the sections
of complicated architecture or machinery, is highly
advantageous to the mind. The parts which we
wish to express, are concealed, and are suggested
partly by the elevation or profile of the figure, and
partly by the connection between the end proposed
in the construction of the building, machine, &c.
and the means which are adapted to effect it.
A knowledge of perspective, is to
be acquired by an operation of the mind directly opposite
to what is necessary in delineating the sections of
bodies; the mind must here be intent only upon the
objects that are delineated upon the retina, exactly
what we see; it must forget or suspend the knowledge
which it has acquired from experience, and must see
with the eye of childhood, no further than the surface.
Every person, who is accustomed to drawing in perspective,
sees external nature, when he pleases, merely as a
picture: this habit contributes much to form
a taste for the fine arts; it may, however, be carried
to excess. There are improvers who prefer the
most dreary ruin to an elegant and convenient mansion,
and who prefer a blasted stump to the glorious foliage
of the oak.
Perspective is not, however, recommended
merely as a means of improving the taste, but as it
is useful in facilitating the knowledge of mechanics.
When once children are familiarly acquainted with
perspective, and with the representations of machines
by elevations, sections, &c. prints will supply them
with an extensive variety of information; and when
they see real machines, their structure and uses will
be easily comprehended. The noise, the seeming
confusion, and the size of several machines, make
it difficult to comprehend and combine their various
parts, without much time, and repeated examination;
the reduced size of prints lays the whole at once
before the eye, and tends to facilitate not only comprehension,
but contrivance. Whoever can delineate progressively
as he invents, saves much labour, much time, and the
hazard of confusion. Various contrivances have
been employed to facilitate drawing in perspective,
as may be seen in “Cabinet de Servier, Mémoires
of the French Academy, Philosophical Transactions,
and lately in the Repertory of Arts.” The
following is simple, cheap, and portable.
PLATE 1. FI.
A B C, three mahogany boards, two,
four, and six inches long, and of the same breadth
respectively, so as to double in the manner represented.
PLATE 1. FI.
The part A is screwed, or clamped
to a table of a convenient height, and a sheet of
paper, one edge of which is put under the piece A,
will be held fast to the table.
The index P is to be set (at pleasure)
with it sharp point to any part of an object which
the eye sees through E, the eye-piece.
The machine is now to be doubled as
in Fi, taking care that the index be not disturbed;
the point, which was before perpendicular, will then
approach the paper horizontally, and the place to which
it points on the paper, must be marked with a pencil.
The machine must be again unfolded, and another point
of the object is to be ascertained in the same manner
as before; the space between these points may be then
connected with a line; fresh points should then be
taken, marked with a pencil, and connected with a
line; and so on successively, until the whole object
is delineated.
Besides the common terms of art, the
technical terms of science should, by degrees, be
rendered familiar to our pupils. Amongst these
the words Space and Time occur, as we have observed,
the soonest, and are of the greatest importance.
Without exact definitions, or abstract reasonings,
a general notion of the use of these terms may be
inculcated by employing them frequently in conversation,
and by applying them to things and circumstances which
occur without preparation, and about which children
are interested, or occupied. “There is
a great space left between the words in that printing.”
The child understands, that space in this sentence
means white paper between black letters. “You
should leave a greater space between the flowers which
you are planting” he knows that you
mean more ground. “There is a great
space between that boat and the ship” space
of water. “I hope the hawk will not be
able to catch that pigeon, there is a great space
between them” space of air. “The
men who are pulling that sack of corn into the granary,
have raised it through half the space between the
door and the ground.” A child cannot be
at any loss for the meaning of the word space in these
or any other practical examples which may occur; but
he should also be used to the word space as a technical
expression, and then he will not be confused or stopped
by a new term when employed in mechanics.
The word time may be used in
the same manner upon numberless occasions to express
the duration of any movement which is performed by
the force of men, or horses, wind, water, or any mechanical
power.
“Did the horses in the mill
we saw yesterday, go as fast as the horses which are
drawing the chaise?” “No, not as fast as
the horses go at present on level ground; but they
went as fast as the chaise-horses do when they go
up hill, or as fast as horses draw a waggon.”
“How many times do the sails
of that wind-mill go round in a minute? Let us
count; I will look at my watch; do you count how often
the sails go round; wait until that broken arm is
uppermost, and when you say now, I will begin
to count the time; when a minute has past, I
will tell you.”
After a few trials, this experiment
will become easy to a child of eight or nine years
old; he may sometimes attend to the watch, and at
other times count the turns of the sails; he may easily
be made to apply this to a horse-mill, or to a water-mill,
a corn-fan, or any machine that has a rotatory motion;
he will be entertained with his new employment; he
will compare the velocities of different machines;
the meaning of this word will be easily added to his
vocabulary.
“Does that part of the arms
of the wind-mill which is near the axle-tree,
or centre, I mean that part which has no cloth
or sail upon it, go as fast as the ends of the arms
that are the farthest from the centre?”
“No, not near so fast.”
“But that part goes as often
round in a minute as the rest of the sail.”
“Yes, but it does not go as fast.”
“How so?”
“It does not go so far round.”
“No, it does not. The extremities
of the sails go through more space in the same
time than the part near the centre.”
By conversations like these, the technical
meaning of the word velocity may be made quite
familiar to a child much younger than what has been
mentioned; he may not only comprehend that velocity
means time and space considered together, but if he
is sufficiently advanced in arithmetic, he may be
readily taught how to express and compare in numbers
velocities composed of certain portions of time
and space. He will not inquire about the abstract
meaning of the word space; he has seen space
measured on paper, on timber, on the water, in the
air, and he perceives distinctly that it is a term
equally applicable to all distances that can exist
between objects of any sort, or that he can see, feel,
or imagine.
Momentum, a less common word, the
meaning of which is not quite so easy to convey to
a child, may, by degrees, be explained to him:
at every instant he feels the effect of momentum in
his own motions, and in the motions of every thing
that strikes against him; his feelings and experience
require only proper terms to become the subject of
his conversation. When he begins to inquire,
it is the proper time to instruct him. For instance,
a boy of ten years old, who had acquired the meaning
of some other terms in science, this morning asked
the meaning of the word momentum; he was desired to
explain what he thought it meant.
He answered, “Force.”
“What do you mean by force?”
“Effort.”
“Of what?”
“Of gravity.”
“Do you mean that force by which a body is drawn
down to the earth?”
“No.”
“Would a feather, if it were
moving with the greatest conceivable swiftness or
velocity, throw down a castle?”
“No."
“Would a mountain torn up by
the roots, as fabled in Milton, if it moved with the
least conceivable velocity, throw down a castle?”
“Yes, I think it would.”
The difference between an uniform,
and an uniformly accelerated motion, the measure of
the velocity of falling bodies, the composition of
motions communicated to the same body in different
directions at the same time, and the cause of the
curvilinear track of projectiles, seem, at first,
intricate subjects, and above the capacity of boys
of ten or twelve years old; but by short and well-timed
lessons, they may be explained without confounding
or fatiguing their attention. We tried another
experiment whilst this chapter was writing, to determine
whether we had asserted too much upon this subject.
After a conversation between two boys upon the descent
of bodies towards the earth, and upon the measure
of the increasing velocity with which they fall, they
were desired, with a view to ascertain whether they
understood what was said, to invent a machine which
should show the difference between an uniform and
an accelerated velocity, and in particular to show,
by occular demonstration, “that if one body moves
in a given time through a given space, with an uniform
motion, and if another body moves through the same
space in the same time with an uniformly accelerated
motion, the uniform motion of the one will be equal
to half the accelerated motion of the other.”
The eldest boy, H , thirteen years
old, invented and executed the following machine for
this purpose:
Plate I, Fi. b is a bracket
9 inches by 5, consisting of a back and two sides
of hard wood: two inches from the back two slits
are made in the sides of the bracket half an inch
deep, and an eighth of an inch wide, to receive the
two wire pivots of a roller; which roller is composed
of a cylinder, three inches long and half an inch
diameter; and a cone three inches long and one inch
diameter in its largest part or base. The cylinder
and cone are not separate, but are turned out of one
piece; a string is fastened to the cone at its base
a, with a bullet or any other small weight at
the other end of it; and another string and weight
are fastened to the cylinder at c; the pivot
p of wire is bent into the form of a handle;
if the handle is turned either way, the strings will
be respectively wound up upon the cone and cylinder;
their lengths should now be adjusted, so that when
the string on the cone is wound up as far as the cone
will permit, the two weights may be at an equal distance
from the bottom of the bracket, which bottom we suppose
to be parallel with the pivots; the bracket should
now be fastened against a wall, at such a height as
to let the weights lightly touch the floor when the
strings are unwound: silk or bobbin is
a proper kind of string for this purpose, as it is
woven or plaited, and therefore is not liable to twist.
When the strings are wound up to their greatest heights,
if the handle be suddenly let go, both the weights
will begin to fall at the same moment; but the weight
1, will descend at first but slowly, and will pass
through but small space compared with the weight 2.
As they descend further, N still continues to
get before N; but after some time, N begins
to overtake N, and at last they come to the ground
together. If this machine is required to show
exactly the space that a falling body would describe
in given times, the cone and cylinder must have grooves
cut spirally upon their circumference, to direct the
string with precision. To describe these spiral
lines, became a new subject of inquiry. The young
mechanics were again eager to exert their powers of
invention; the eldest invented a machine upon the
same principle as that which is used by the best workmen
for cutting clock fusees; and it is described in Berthoud.
The youngest invented the engine delineated, Plate
1, Fi.
The roller or cone (or both together)
which it is required to cut spirally, must be furnished
with a handle, and a toothed wheel w, which
turns a smaller wheel or pinion w. This
pinion carries with it a screw s, which draws
forward the puppet p, in which the graver of
chisel g slides without shake. This
graver has a point or edge shaped properly to form
the spiral groove, with a shoulder to regulate the
depth of the groove. The iron rod r, which
is firmly fastened in the puppet, slides through mortices
at mm, and guides the puppet in a straight
line.
The rest of the machine is intelligible from the drawing.
A simple method of showing the nature
of compound forces was thought of at the same time.
An ivory ball was placed at the corner of a board
sixteen inches broad, and two feet long; two other
similar balls were let fall down inclined troughs
against the first ball in different directions, but
at the same time. One fell in a direction parallel
to the length of the board; the other ball fell back
in a direction parallel to its breadth. By raising
the troughs, such a force was communicated to each
of the falling balls, as was sufficient to drive the
ball that was at rest to that side or end of the board
which was opposite, or at right angles, to the line
of its motion.
When both balls were let fall together,
they drove the ball that was at rest diagonally, so
as to reach the opposite corner. If the same
board were placed as an inclined plane, at an angle
of five or six degrees, a ball placed at one of its
uppermost corners, would fall with an accelerated
motion in a direct line; but if another ball were
made (by descending through an inclined trough) to
strike the first ball at right angles to the line
of its former descent, at the moment when it began
to descend, it would not, as in the former experiment,
move diagonally, but would describe a curve.
The reason why it describes a curve,
and why that curve is not circular, was easily understood.
Children who are thus induced to invent machines or
apparatus for explaining and demonstrating the laws
of mechanism, not only fix indelibly those laws in
their own minds, but enlarge their powers of invention,
and preserve a certain originality of thought, which
leads to new discoveries.
We therefore strongly recommend it
to teachers, to use as few precepts as possible in
the rudiments of science, and to encourage their pupils
to use their own understandings as they advance.
In mechanism, a general view of the powers and uses
of engines is all that need be taught; where more
is necessary, such a foundation, with the assistance
of good books, and the examination of good machinery,
will perfect the knowledge of theory and facilitate
practice.
At first we should not encumber our
pupils with accurate demonstration. The application
of mathematics to mechanics is undoubtedly of the
highest use, and has opened a source of ingenious
and important inquiry. Archimedes, the greatest
name amongst mechanic philosophers, scorned the mere
practical application of his sublime discoveries,
and at the moment when the most stupendous effects
were producing by his engines, he was so deeply absorbed
in abstract speculation as to be insensible to the
fear of death. We do not mean, therefore, to
undervalue either the application of strict demonstration
to problems in mechanics, or the exhibition of the
most accurate machinery in philosophical lectures;
but we wish to point out a method of giving a general
notion of the mechanical organs to our pupils, which
shall be immediately obvious to their comprehension,
and which may serve as a sure foundation for future
improvement. We are told by a vulgar proverb,
that though we believe what we see, we have yet a
higher belief in what we feel. This adage
is particularly applicable to mechanics. When
a person perceives the effect of his own bodily exertions
with different engines, and when he can compare in
a rough manner their relative advantages, he is not
disposed to reject their assistance, or expect more
than is reasonable from their application. The
young theorist in mechanics thinks he can produce a
perpetual motion! When he has been accustomed
to refer to the plain dictates of common sense and
experience, on this, as well as on every other subject,
he will not easily be led astray by visionary theories.
To bring the sense of feeling to our
assistance in teaching the uses of the mechanic powers,
the following apparatus was constructed, to which
we have given the name Panorganon.
It is composed of two principal parts:
a frame to contain the moving machinery; and a capstan
or windlass, which is erected on a sill
or plank, that is sunk a few inches into the ground:
the frame is by this means, and by six braces or props,
rendered steady. The cross rail, or transom,
is strengthened by braces and a king-post to make
it lighter and cheaper. The capstan consists
of an upright shaft, upon which are fixed two drums;
about which a rope may be wound up, and two levers
or arms by which it may be turned round. There
is also a screw of iron coiled round the lower part
of the shaft, to show the properties of the screw
as a mechanic power. The rope which goes round
the drum passes over one of the pulleys near
to the top of the frame, and under another pulley
near the bottom of the frame. As two drums
of different sizes are employed, it is necessary to
have an upright roller to conduct the rope in a proper
direction to the pulleys, when either of the drums
is used. Near the frame, and in the direction
in which the rope runs, is laid a platform or road
of deal boards, one board in breadth, and twenty or
thirty feet long, upon which a small sledge loaded
with different weights may be drawn. Plate 2.
Fi.
F. F. The frame.
b. b. Braces to keep the frame steady.
a. a. a. Angular braces to strengthen
the transom; and also a king-post.
S. A round, taper shaft, strengthened
above and below the mortises with iron hoops.
L L. Two arms, or levers, by which
the shaft, &c. are to be moved round.
D D. The drum, which has two rims
of different circumferences.
R. The roller to conduct the rope.
P. The pulley, round which the rope passes to the
larger drum.
P 2. Another pulley to answer to the smaller
drum.
P 3. A pulley through which the
rope passes when experiments are tried with levers,
&c.
P 4. Another pulley through which
the rope passes when the sledge is used.
Ro. The road of deal boards for the sledge to
move on.
Sl. The sledge, with pieces of
hard wood attached to it, to guide it on the road.
Uses of the Panorganon.
As this machine is to be moved by
the force of men or children, and as their force varies
not only with the strength and weight of each individual,
but also according to the different manner in which
that strength or weight is applied; it is, in the
first place, requisite to establish one determinate
mode of applying human force to the machine; and also
a method of determining the relative force of each
individual whose strength is applied to it.
To estimate the force with which
a person can draw horizontally by a rope over his
shoulder.
Hang a common long scale-beam (without
scales or chains) from the top or transom of
the frame, so as that one end of it may come within
an inch of one side or post of the machine. Tie
a rope to the hook of the scale-beam, where the chains
of the scale are usually hung, and pass it through
the pulley P 3, which is about four feet from the ground;
let the person pull this rope from 1 towards 2, turning
his back to the machine, and pulling the rope over
his shoulder P. Fi. As
the pulley may be either too high or too low to permit
the rope to be horizontal, the person who pulls it
should be placed ten or fifteen feet from the machine,
which will lessen the angular direction of the cord,
and the inaccuracy of the experiment. Hang weights
to the other end of the scale-beam, until the person
who pulls can but just walk forward, pulling fairly
without propping his feet against any thing.
This weight will estimate the force with which he can
draw horizontally by a rope over his shoulder.
Let a child who tries this, walk on the board with
dry shoes; let him afterwards chalk his shoes, and
afterwards try it with his shoes soaped: he will
find that he can pull with different degrees of force
in these different circumstances; but when he tries
the following experiments, let his shoes be always
dry, that his force may be always the same.
To show the power of the three
different sorts of levers.
Instead of putting the cord that comes
from the scale-beam, as in the last experiment, over
the shoulder of the boy, hook it to the end 1 of the
lever L, Fi. Plate 2. This lever is passed
through a socket Plate 2. Fi. in
which it can be shifted from one of its ends towards
the other, and can be fastened at any place by the
screw of the socket. This socket has two gudgeons,
upon which it, and the lever which it contains, can
turn. This socket and its gudgeons can be lifted
out of the holes in which it plays, between the rail
R R, Plate 2. Fi. and may be put into other
holes at R R, Fi. Loop another rope to the
other end of this lever, and let the boy pull as before.
Perhaps it should be pointed out, that the boy must
walk in a direction contrary to that in which he walked
before, viz. from 1 towards 3. The height
to which the weight ascends, and the distance to which
the boy advances, should be carefully marked and measured;
and it will be found, that he can raise the weight
to the same height, advancing through the same space
as in the former experiment. In this case, as
both ends of the lever moved through equal spaces,
the lever only changed the direction of the motion,
and added no mechanical power to the direct strength
of the boy.
Shift the lever to its extremity in
the socket; the middle of the lever will be
now opposite to the pulley, P. Fi. hook
to it the rope that goes through the pulley P 3, and
fasten to the other end of the lever the rope by which
the boy is to pull. This will be a lever of
the second kind, as it is called in books of mechanics;
in using which, the resistance is placed between
the centre of motion or fulcrum, and the moving power.
He will now raise double the weight that he did in
Experiment II, and he will advance through double the
space.
Shift the lever, and the socket which
forms the axis (without shifting the lever from the
place in which it was in the socket in the last experiment)
to the holes that are prepared for it at R R, Plate
2. Fi. The free end of the lever E will
now be opposite to the rope, and to the pulley (over
which the rope comes from the scale-beam.) Hook this
rope to it, and hook the rope by which the boy pulls,
to the middle of the lever. The effect will now
be different from what it was in the two last experiments;
the boy will advance only half as far, and will raise
only half as much weight as before. This is called
a lever of the third sort. The first and
second kinds of levers are used in quarrying; and
the operations of many tools may be referred to them.
The third kind of lever is employed but seldom, but
its properties may be observed with advantage whilst
a long ladder is raised, as the man who raises it,
is obliged to exert an increasing force until the
ladder is nearly perpendicular. When this lever
is used, it is obvious, from what has been said, that
the power must always pass through less space than
the thing which is to be moved; it can never, therefore,
be of service in gaining power. But the object
of some machines, is to increase velocity, instead
of obtaining power, as in a sledge-hammer moved by
mill-work. (V. the plates in Emerson’s Mechanics,
N.)
The experiments upon levers may be
varied at pleasure, increasing or diminishing the
mechanical advantage, so as to balance the power and
the resistance, to accustom the learners to calculate
the relation between the power and the effect in different
circumstances; always pointing out, that whatever
excess there is in the power, or in the resistance,
is always compensated by the difference of space through
which the inferiour passes.
The experiments which we have mentioned,
are sufficiently satisfactory to a pupil, as to the
immediate relation between the power and the resistance;
but the different spaces through which the power and
the resistance move when one exceeds the other, cannot
be obvious, without they pass through much larger
spaces than levers will permit.
Place the sledge on the farthest end
of the wooden road Plate 2. Fi. fasten
a rope to the sledge, and conduct it through the lowest
pulley P 4, and through the pulley P 3, so as that
the boy may be enabled to draw it by the rope passed
over his shoulder. The sledge must now be loaded,
until the boy can but just advance with short steps
steadily upon the wooden road; this must be done with
care, as there will be but just room for him beside
the rope. He will meet the sledge exactly on
the middle of the road, from which he must step aside
to pass the sledge. Let the time of this experiment
be noted. It is obvious that the boy and the
sledge move with equal velocity; there is, therefore,
no mechanical advantage obtained by the pulleys.
The weight that he can draw will be about half a hundred,
if he weigh about nine stone; but the exact force
with which the boy draws, is to be known by Experiment
I.
The wheel and axle.
This organ is usually called in mechanics,
The axis in peritrochio. A hard
name, which might well be spared, as the word windlass
or capstan would convey a more distinct idea to our
pupils.
To the largest drum, Plate 2.
Fi. fasten a cord, and pass it through the pulley
P downwards, and through the pulley P 4 to the sledge
placed at the end of the wooden road, which is farthest
from the machine. Let the boy, by a rope fastened
to the extremity of one of the arms of the capstan,
and passed over his shoulder, draw the capstan round;
he will wind the rope round the drum, and draw the
sledge upon its road. To make the sledge advance
twenty-four feet upon its road, the boy must have
walked circularly 144 feet, which is six times as
far, and he will be able to draw about three hundred
weight, which is six times as much as in the last
experiment.
It may now be pointed out, that the
difference of space, passed through by the power in
this experiment, is exactly equal to the difference
of weight, which the boy could draw without the capstan.
Let the rope be now attached to the
smaller drum; the boy will draw nearly twice as much
weight upon the sledge as before, and will go through
double the space.
Where there are a number of boys,
let five or six of them, whose power of drawing (estimated
as in Experiment I) amounts to six times as much as
the force of the boy at the capstan, pull at the end
of the rope which was fastened to the sledge;
they will balance the force of the boy at the capstan:
either they, or he, by a sudden pull, may advance,
but if they pull fairly, there will be no advantage
on either part. In this experiment, the rope
should pass through the pulley P 3, and should be
coiled round the larger drum. And it must be also
observed, that in all experiments upon the motion
of bodies, in which there is much friction, as where
a sledge is employed, the results are never so uniform
as in other circumstances.
The Pulley.
Upon the pulley we shall say little,
as it is in every body’s hands, and experiments
may be tried upon it without any particular apparatus.
It should, however, be distinctly inculcated, that
the power is not increased by a fixed pulley.
For this purpose, a wheel without a rim, or, to speak
with more propriety, a number of spokes fixed in a
nave, should be employed. (Plate 2. Fi.)
Pieces like the heads of crutches should be fixed
at the ends of these spokes, to receive a piece of
girth-web, which is used instead of a cord, because
a cord would be unsteady; and a strap of iron with
a hook to it should play upon the centre, by which
it may at times be suspended, and from which at other
times a weight may be hung.
Let the skeleton of a pulley be hung
by the iron strap from the transom of the frame; fasten
a piece of web to one of the radii, and another to
the end of the opposite radius. If two boys of
equal weight pull these pieces of girth-web, they
will balance each other; or two equal weights hung
to these webs, will be in equilibrio. If
a piece of girth-web be put round the uppermost radius,
two equal weights hung at the ends of it will remain
immoveable; but if either of them be pulled, or if
a small additional weight be added to either of them,
it will descend, and the web will apply itself successively
to the ascending radii, and will detach itself from
those that are descending. If this movement be
carefully considered, it will be perceived, that the
web, in unfolding itself, acts in the same manner
upon the radii as two ropes would if they were hung
to the extremities of the opposite radii in succession.
The two radii which are opposite, may be considered
as a lever of the first sort, where the centre is in
the middle of the lever; as each end moves through
an equal space, there is no mechanical advantage.
But if this skeleton-pulley be employed as a common
block or tackle, its motions and properties
will be entirely different.
Nail a piece of girth-web to a post,
at the distance of three or four feet from the ground;
fasten the other end of it to one of the radii.
Fasten another piece of web to the opposite radius,
and let a boy hold the skeleton-pulley suspended by
the web; hook weights to the strap that hangs from
the centre. The end of the radius to which the
fixed girth-web is fastened, will remain immoveable;
but, if the boy pulls the web which he holds in his
hand upwards, he will be able to lift nearly double
the weight, which he can raise from the ground by a
simple rope, without the machine, and he will perceive
that his hand moves through twice as great a space
as the weight ascends: he has, therefore, the
mechanical advantage which he would have by a lever
of the second sort, as in Experiment III. Let
a piece of web be put round the under radii, let one
end of it be nailed to the post, and the other be
held by the boy, and it will represent the application
of a rope to a moveable pulley; if its motion be carefully
considered, it will appear that the radii, as they
successively apply themselves to the web, represent
a series of levers of the second kind. A pulley
is nothing more than an infinite number of such levers;
the cord at one end of the diameter serving as a fulcrum
for the organ during its progress. If
this skeleton-pulley be used horizontally, instead
of perpendicularly, the circumstances which have been
mentioned, will appear more obvious.
Upon the wooden road lay down a piece
of girth-web; nail one end of it to the road; place
the pulley upon the web at the other end of the board,
and, bringing the web over the radii, let the boy,
taking hold of it, draw the loaded sledge fastened
to the hook at the centre of the pulley: he will
draw nearly twice as much in this manner as he could
without the pulley.
Here the web lying on the road, shows
more distinctly, that it is quiescent where the lowest
radius touches it; and if the radii, as they tread
upon it, are observed, their points will appear at
rest, whilst the centre of the pulley will go as fast
as the sledge, and the top of each radius successively
(and the boy’s hand which unfolds the web) will
move twice as fast as the centre of the pulley and
the sledge.
If a person, holding a stick in his
hand, observes the relative motions of the top, and
the middle, and the bottom of the stick, whilst he
inclines it, he will see that the bottom of the stick
has no motion on the ground, and that the middle has
only half the motion of the top. This property
of the pulley has been dwelt upon, because it elucidates
the motion of a wheel rolling upon the ground; and
it explains a common paradox, which appears at first
inexplicable. “The bottom of a rolling
wheel never moves upon the road.”
This is asserted only of a wheel moving over hard
ground, which, in fact, may be considered rather as
laying down its circumference upon the road, than
as moving upon it.
The inclined Plane and the Wedge.
The inclined plane is to be
next considered. When a heavy body is to be raised,
it is often convenient to lay a sloping artificial
road of planks, up which it may be pushed or drawn.
This mechanical power, however, is but of little service
without the assistance of wheels or rollers; we shall,
therefore, speak of it as it is applied in another
manner, under the name of the wedge, which is,
in fact, a moving inclined plane; but if it is required
to explain the properties of the inclined plane by
the panorganon, the wooden road may be raised and
set to any inclination that is required, and the sledge
may be drawn upon it as in the former experiments.
Let one end of a lever, N. Plate 2.
Fi. with a wheel at one end of it, be hinged to
the post of the frame, by means of a gudgeon driven
or screwed into the post. To prevent this lever
from deviating sideways, let a slip of wood be connected
with it by a nail, which shall be fast in the lever,
but which moves freely in a hole in the rail.
The other end of this slip must be fastened to a stake
driven into the ground at three or four feet from
the lever, at one side of it, and towards the end
in which the wheel is fixed (Plate 2. Fig 10.
which is a vue d’oiseau) in the same manner
as the treadle of a common lathe is managed, and as
the treadle of a loom is sometimes guided.
Under the wheel of this lever place
an inclined plane or half-wedge (Plate 2. Fi.) on the wooden road, with rollers under it, to
prevent friction; fasten a rope to the foremost
end of the wedge, and pass it through the pulleys
(P 4. and P 3.) as in the fifth experiment. Let
a boy draw the sledge by this rope over his shoulder,
and he will find, that as it advances it will raise
the weight upwards; the wedge is five feet long, and
elevated one foot. Now, if the perpendicular
ascent of the weight, and the space through which he
advances, be compared, he will find, that the space
through which he has passed will be five times as
great as that through which the weight has ascended;
and that this wedge has enabled him to raise
five times as much as he could raise without it, if
his strength were applied, as in Experiment I, without
any mechanical advantage. By making this wedge
in two parts hinged together, with a graduated piece
to keep them asunder, the wedge may be adjusted to
any given obliquity; and it will be always found,
that the mechanical advantage of the wedge may be
ascertained by comparing its perpendicular elevation
with its base. If the base of the wedge is 2,
3, 4, 5, or any other number of times greater than
its height, it will enable the boy to raise respectively
2, 3, 4, or 5 times more weight than he could do in
Experiment I, by which his power is estimated.
The Screw.
The screw is an inclined plane
wound round a cylinder; the height of all its revolutions
round the cylinder taken together, compared with the
space through which the power that turns it passes,
is the measure of its mechanical advantage.
Let the lever, used in the last experiment, be turned
in such a manner as to reach from its gudgeon to the
shaft of the Panorganon, guided by an attendant lever
as before. (Plate 2. Fi.) Let the wheel
rest upon the lowest helix or thread of the
screw: as the arms of the shaft are turned round,
the wheel will ascend, and carry up the weight which
is fastened to the lever. As the situation of
the screw prevents the weight from being suspended
exactly from the centre of the screw, proper allowance
must be made for this in estimating the force of the
screw, or determining the mechanical advantage gained
by the lever: this can be done by measuring the
perpendicular ascent of the weight, which in all cases
is better, and more expeditious, than measuring the
parts of a machine, and estimating its force by calculation;
because the different diameters of ropes, and other
small circumstances, are frequently mistaken in estimates.
The space passed through by the moving
power, and by that which it moves, are infallible
data for estimating the powers of engines. Two
material subjects of experiments, yet remain for the
Panorganon; friction, and wheels of carriages:
but we have already extended this article far beyond
its just proportion to similar chapters in this work.
We repeat, that it is not intended in this, or in any
other part of our design, to write treatises upon
science; but merely to point out methods for initiating
young people in the rudiments of knowledge, and of
giving them a clear and distinct view of those principles
upon which they are founded. No preceptor, who
has had experience, will cavil at the superficial
knowledge of a boy of twelve or thirteen upon these
subjects; he will perceive, that the general view,
which we wish to give our pupils of the useful arts
and sciences, must certainly tend to form a taste
for literature and investigation. The sciolist
has learned only to talk we wish
to teach our pupils to think, upon the various
objects of human speculation.
The Panorganon may be employed in
trying the resistance of air and water; the force
of different muscles; and in a great variety of amusing
and useful experiments. In academies, and private
families, it may be erected in the place allotted
for amusement, where it will furnish entertainment
for many a vacant hour. When it has lost its
novelty, the shaft may from time to time be taken down,
and a swing may be suspended in its place. It
may be constructed at the expense of five or six pounds:
that which stands before our window, was made for
less than three guineas, as we had many of the materials
beside us for other purposes.