Revival of crystal-gazing. Antiquity
of the practice. Its general harmlessness.
Superstitious explanations. Crystal-gazing and
‘illusions hypnagogiques’. Visualisers.
Poetic vision. Ancient and savage practices
analogous to crystal-gazing. New Zealand.
North America. Egypt. Sir Walter’s
interest in the subject. Mr. Kinglake.
Greek examples. Dr. Dee. Miss X. Another
modern instance. Successes and failures.
Revival of lost memories. Possible thought-transference.
Inferences from antiquity and diffusion of practice.
Based on actual experience. Anecdotes of Dr.
Gregory. Children as visionaries. Not to
be encouraged.
The practice of ‘scrying,’
‘peeping,’ or ‘crystal-gazing,’
has been revived in recent years, and is, perhaps,
the only ‘occult’ diversion which may
be free from psychological or physical risk, and which
it is easy not to mix with superstition. The
antiquity and world-wide diffusion of scrying, in
one form or other, interests the student of human
nature. Meanwhile the comparatively few persons
who can see pictures in a clear depth, may be as innocently
employed while so doing, as if they were watching
the clouds, or the embers. ‘May be,’
one must say, for crystal-seers are very apt to fall
back on our old friend, the animistic hypothesis,
and to explain what they see, or fancy they see, by
the theory that ‘spirits’ are at the bottom
of it all. In Mrs. de Morgan’s work From
Matter to Spirit, suggestions of this kind are not
absent: ’As an explanation of crystal-seeing,
a spiritual drawing was once made, representing a
spirit directing on the crystal a stream of influence,’
and so forth. Mrs. de Morgan herself seemed
rather to hold that the act of staring at a crystal
mesmerises the observer. The person who looks
at it often becomes sleepy. ’Sometimes
the eyes close, at other times tears flow.’
People who become sleepy, or cry, or get hypnotised,
will probably consult their own health and comfort
by leaving crystal balls alone.
There are others, however, who are
no more hypnotised by crystal-gazing than tea-drinking,
or gardening, or reading a book, and who can still
enjoy visions as beautiful as those of the opium eater,
without any of the reaction. Their condition
remains perfectly normal, that is, they are wide awake
to all that is going on. In some way their fancy
is enlivened, and they can behold, in the glass, just
such vivid pictures as many persons habitually see
between sleeping and waking, illusions hypnagogiques.
These ‘hypnagogic illusions’ Pontus de
Tyard described in a pretty sonnet, more than three
hundred years ago. Maury, in his book on dreams
has recorded, and analysed them. They represent
faces, places, a page of print, a flame of fire, and
so forth, and it is one of their peculiarities that
the faces rapidly shift and alter, generally from
beautiful to ugly. A crystal-seer seems to be
a person who can see, in a glass, while awake and
with open eyes, visions akin to those which perhaps
the majority of people see with shut eyes, between
sleeping and waking. It seems probable that
people who, when they think, see a mental picture
of the subject of their thoughts, people who are good
‘visualisers,’ are likely to succeed best
with the crystal, some of them can ‘visualise’
purposely, in the crystal, while others cannot.
Many who are very bad ‘visualisers,’ like
the writer, who think in words, not in pictures, see
bright and distinct hypnagogic illusions, yet see
nothing in the crystal, however long they stare at
it. And there are crystal-seers who are not subject
to hypnagogic illusions. These facts, like the
analogous facts of the visualisation of arithmetical
figures, analysed by Mr. Galton, show interesting
varieties in the conduct of mental operations.
Thus we speak of ‘vision’ in a poet, or
novelist, and it seems likely that men of genius ‘see’
their fictitious characters and landscapes, while
people of critical temperament, if they attempt creative
work, are conscious that they do not create, but construct.
On the other hand many incompetent novelists are convinced
that they have ‘vision,’ that they see
and hear their characters, but they do not, as genius
does, transfer the ‘vision’ to their readers.
This is a digression from the topic
of hallucinations caused by gazing into a clear depth.
Forms of crystal-gazing‚ it is well known‚ are found
among savages. The New Zealanders‚ according
to Taylor‚ gaze in a drop of blood‚ as the Egyptians
do in a drop of ink. In North America‚ the Pere
lé Jeune found that a kind of thought reading
was practised thus: it was believed that a sick
person had certain desires‚ if these could be gratified‚
he would recover. The sorcerers‚ therefore‚
gazed into water in a bowl expecting to see there
visions of the desired objects. The Egyptian
process with the boy and the ink‚ is too familiar to
need description. In Scott’s Journal (i) we read of the excitement which the reports of
Lord Prudhoe and Colonel Felix‚ caused among
the curious. A boy‚ selected by these English
gentlemen‚ saw and described Shakspeare‚ and Colonel
Felix’s brother‚ who had lost an arm.
The ceremonies of fumigation‚ and the preliminary visions
of flags‚ and a sultan‚ are not necessary in modern
crystal-gazing. Scott made inquiries at Malta‚
and wished to visit Alexandria. He was attracted‚
doubtless‚ by the resemblance to Dr. Dee’s tales
of his magic ball‚ and to the legends of his own Aunt
Margaret’s Mirror. The Quarterly Review offers an explanation
which explains nothing. The experiments of Mr.
Lane were tolerably successful‚ those of Mr. Kinglake‚
in Eothen‚ were amusingly the reverse. Dr. Keate‚
the flogging headmaster of Eton‚ was described by
the seer as a beautiful girl‚ with golden hair and
blue eyes. The modern explanation of successes
would apparently be that the boy does‚ occasionally‚
see the reflection of his interrogator’s thoughts.
In a paper in the Proceedings of the
Society for Psychical Research (part xiv.), an anonymous
writer gives the results of some historical investigation
into the antiquities of crystal-gazing. The stories
of cups, ‘wherein my lord divines,’ like
Joseph, need not necessarily indicate gazing into
the deeps of the cup. There were other modes
of using cups and drops of wine, not connected with
visions. At Patrae, in Greece, Pausanias describes
the dropping of a mirror on to the surface of a well,
the burning of incense, and the vision of the patient
who consults the oracle in the deeps of the mirror.
A Christian Father asserts that, in some cases,
a basin with a glass bottom was used, through which
the gazer saw persons concealed in a room below, and
took them for real visions. In mirror-magic
(catoptromancy), the child seer’s eyes were
bandaged, and he saw with the top of his head!
The Specularii continued the tradition through the
Middle Ages, and, in the sixteenth century Dr. Dee
ruined himself by his infatuation for ‘show-stones,’
in which Kelly saw, or pretended to see, visions which
Dr. Dee interpreted. Dee kept voluminous diaries
of his experiments, part of which is published in
a folio by Meric Casaubon. The work is flighty,
indeed crazy; Dee thought that the hallucinations
were spirits, and believed that his ‘show-stones’
were occasionally spirited away by the demons.
Kelly pretended to hear noises in the stones, and
to receive messages.
In our own time, while many can see
pictures, few know what the pictures represent.
Some explain them by interpreting the accompanying
‘raps,’ or by ‘automatic writing’.
The intelligence thus conveyed is then found to exist
in county histories, newspapers, and elsewhere, a
circumstance which lends itself to interpretation
of more sorts than one. Without these very dubious
modes of getting at the meaning of the crystal pictures,
they remain, of course, mere picturesque hallucinations.
The author of the paper referred to, is herself a
crystal-seer, and (in Borderland N mentions one
very interesting vision. She and a friend stared
into one of Dr. Dee’s ‘show-stones,’
at the Stuart exhibition, and both beheld the same
scene, not a scene they could have guessed at, which
was going on at the seer’s own house. As
this writer, though versed in hallucinations, entirely
rejects any ‘spiritual’ theory, and conceives
that, she is dealing with purely psychological curiosities,
her evidence is the better worth notice, and may be
compared with that of a crystal-seer for whose evidence
the present writer can vouch, as far as one mortal
may vouch for that of another.
Miss X., the writer in the Psychical
Proceedings, has been able to see pictures in crystals
and other polished surfaces, or, indeed, independently
of these, since childhood. She thinks that the
visions are:
1. After-images, or recrudescent
memories (often memories of things not consciously
noted).
2. Objectivations of ideas
or images, consciously or unconsciously present to
the mind.
3. Visions, possibly telepathic
or clairvoyant, implying acquirement of knowledge
by supernormal means. The first class is much
the most frequent in this lady’s experience.
She can occasionally refresh her memory by looking
into the crystal.
The other seer, known to the writer,
cannot do this, and her pictures, as far as she knows,
are purely fanciful. Perhaps an ‘automatic
writer’ might interpret them, in the rather dubious
manner of that art. As far as the ‘scryer’
knows, however, her pictures of places and people
are not revivals of memory. For example, she
sees an ancient ship, with a bird’s beak for
prow, come into harbour, and behind it a man carrying
a crown. This is a mere fancy picture.
On one occasion she saw a man, like an Oriental priest,
with a white caftan, contemplating the rise and fall
of a fountain of fire: suddenly, at the summit
of the fire, appeared a human hand, pointing downwards,
to which the old priest looked up. This was in
August, 1893. Later in the month the author happened
to take up, at Loch Sheil, Lady Burton’s Life
of Sir Richard Burton. On the back of the cover
is a singular design in gold. A woman in widow’s
weeds is bowing beneath rays of light, over which appears
a human hand, marked R. F. B. on the wrist.
The author at once wrote asking his friend the crystal-gazer
if she had seen this work of art, which might have
unconsciously suggested the picture. The lady,
however, was certain that she had not seen the Life
of Sir Richard Burton, though her eye, of course,
may have fallen on it in a bookseller’s shop,
while her mind did not consciously take it in.
If this was a revival of a sub-conscious memory in
the crystal, it was the only case of that process
in her experience.
On the other hand Miss X. can trace
many of her visions to memories, as Maury could in
his illusions hypnagogiques. Thus, Miss X. saw
in the crystal, the printed announcement of a friend’s
death. She had not consciously read the Times,
but remembered that she had held it up before her
face as a firescreen. This kind of revival, as
she says, corresponds to the writing, with planchette,
of scraps from the Chanson de Roland, by a person
who had never consciously read a line of it,
and who did not even know what stratum of Old French
was represented by the fragments. Miss X. seems
not to know either; for she calls it ‘Provencal’.
Similar instances of memory revived are not very
uncommon in dreams. Miss X. can consciously put
a group of fanciful characters into the crystal, while
this is beyond the power of the seer known to the
writer, who has attempted to perceive what a friend
is doing at a distance, but with no success.
Thus she tried to discover what the writer might be
about, and secured a view of two large sunny rooms,
with a shadowy figure therein. Now it is very
probable that the writer was in just such a room,
at – Castle, but the seer saw, on
the library table, a singular mirror, which did not
exist there, and a model of a castle, also non-existent.
The knowledge that the person sought for was staying
at a ‘castle,’ may have unconsciously suggested
this model in the picture.
A pretty case of revived memory is
given by Miss X. She wanted the date of Ptolemy Philadelphus.
Later, in the crystal, she saw a conventional old
Jew, writing in a book with massive clasps. Using
a magnifying glass, she found that he was writing Greek,
but the lines faded, and she only saw the Roman numerals
LXX. These suggested the seventy Hebrews who
wrote the Septuagint, with the date, 277 B.C., which
served for Ptolemy Philadelphus. Miss X. later
remembered a memoria technica which she had once
learned, with the clue, ‘Now Jewish elders indite
a Greek copy’. It is obvious that these
queer symbolical reawakenings of memory explain much
of the (apparently) ‘unknown’ information
given by ‘ghosts,’ and in dreams.
A lady, who had long been in very bad health, was
one evening seized by a violent recrudescence of memory,
and for hours poured out the minutest details of the
most trivial occurrences; the attack was followed
by a cerebral malady from which she fortunately recovered.
The same phenomenon of awakened memory has occasionally
been reported by people who were with difficulty restored
after being seven-eighths drowned.
The crystal ball, in the proper hands,
merely illustrates the possibility of artificially
reviving memory, while the fanciful visions, akin
to illusions hypnagogiques, have, in all ages, been
interpreted by superstition as revelations of the distant
or the future. Of course, if there is such a
thing as occasional transference of thought, so that
the idea in the inquirer’s mind is reflected
in the crystal-gazer’s vision, the hypothesis
of the superstitious will fix on this as a miracle,
still more will that hypothesis be strengthened, if
future or distant events, not consciously known, are
beheld. Such things must occasionally occur,
by chance, in the myriad confusions of dreams, and,
to the same extent, in crystal visions. Miss
X.’s three cases of possible telepathy in her
own experience are trivial, and do not seem to rise
beyond the possibility of fortuitous coincidence:
and her possible clairvoyant visions she leaves to
the judgment of the reader, ’to interpret as
clairvoyance, or coincidence, or prevision, or whatever
else he will’. The crystal-gazer known
to the author once managed to see the person (unknown
to her) who was in the mind of the other party in
the experiment. But she has made scarcely any
experiments of this description.
The inferences to be drawn from crystal-gazing
are not unimportant. First, we note that the
practice is very ancient and widely diffused, among
civilised and uncivilised people. In this diffusion
it answers to the other practices, the magical rites
of Australian blacks, Greeks, Eskimo; to the stories
of ‘death-bed wraiths,’ of rappings, and
so forth. Now this uniformity, as far as regards
the latter phenomena, may be explained by transmission
of ideas, or by the uniformity of human nature, while
the phenomena themselves may be mere inventions like
other myths. In the case of crystal-gazing,
however, we can scarcely push scepticism so far as
to deny that the facts exist, that hallucinations
are actually provoked. The inference is that
a presumption is raised in favour of the actuality
of the other phenomena universally reported.
They, too, may conceivably be hallucinatory; the rappings
and haunting noises may be auditory, as the crystal
visions are ocular hallucinations. The sounds
so widely attested may not cause vibrations in the
air, just as the visions are not really in
the crystal ball. As the unconscious self suggests
the pictures in the ball, so it may suggest the unexplained
noises. But while, as a rule, only one gazer
sees the visions, the sounds (usually but not invariably)
are heard by all present. On the whole, the
one case wherein we find facts, if only facts of hallucination,
at the bottom of the belief in a world-wide and world-old
practice, rather tends in the direction of belief
in the other facts, not less universally alleged.
We know too much about mythology to agree with Dr.
Johnson, in holding that ’a belief, which prevails
as far as human nature is diffused, could become universal
only by its truth,’ that ’those who never
heard of one another would not have agreed in a tale
which nothing but experience could make credible’.
But, on the other hand, a belief is not necessarily
untrue, because it is universally diffused.
In the second place, crystal-gazing
shows how a substratum of fact may be so overlaid
with mystic mummeries, incantations, fumigations,
pentacles: and so overwhelmed in superstitious
interpretations, introducing fairies and spirits,
that the facts run the risk of being swept away in
the litter and dust of nonsense. Science has
hardly thought crystal-gazing worthy even of contempt,
yet it appears to deserve the notice of psychologists.
To persons who can ‘scry,’ and who do
not see hideous illusions, or become hypnotised, or
superstitious, or incur headaches, scrying is a harmless
gateway into Les Paradis Artificiels. ’And
the rest, they may live and learn.’
A very few experiments will show people
whether they are scryers, or not. The phenomena,
it seems, are usually preceded by a mistiness, or
milkiness, of the glass: this clears off, and
pictures appear. Even the best scryers often
fail to see anything in the crystal which maintains
its natural ‘diaphaneity,’ as Dr. Dee says.
Thus the conditions under which the scryer can scry,
are, as yet, unascertained.
The phenomena of scrying were not
unknown to Dr. Gregory, Professor of Chemistry in
the University of Edinburgh. Dr. Gregory believed
in ‘odylic fluid’ on the evidence of Reichenbach’s
experiments, which nobody seems to have repeated successfully
under strict tests. Clairvoyance also was part
of Dr. Gregory’s faith, and, to be fair, phenomena
were exhibited at his house, in the presence of a learned
and distinguished witness known to the writer, which
could only be accounted for either by thought transference,
or by an almost, or quite incredible combination of
astuteness, and imposture on the side of Dr. Gregory
himself. In presence of the clairvoyants
the nobleman of whom we speak thought not of his own
house, but of a room in the house of a friend.
It possessed a very singular feature which it is
needless to describe here, but which was entirely out
of the experience of the clairvoyante.
She described it, however, expressing astonishment
at what she ‘saw’. This, unless Dr.
Gregory guessed what was likely to be thought of,
and was guilty of collusion, can only be explained
by thought transference. In other cases the
doctor was convinced that he had evidence of actual
clairvoyance, and it is difficult to estimate the amount
of evidence which will clear such a belief of the
charge of credulity. As to ‘scrying’
the doctor thought it could be done in ‘mesmerised
water,’ water bewitched. There is no reason
to imagine that ‘mesmerised’ is different
from ordinary water. He knew that folklore retained
the belief in scrying in crystal balls, and added some
superfluous magical incantations. The doctor
himself was lucky enough to buy an old magical crystal
in which some boys, after long staring, saw persons
unknown to themselves, but known to the professor,
and also persons known to neither. A little
girl, casually picking up a crystal ball, cried, ’There’s
a ship in it, with its cloth all in rags. Now
it tumbles down, and a woman is working at it, and
holds her head in her hand.’ This is a
very fair example of a crystal fancy picture.
The child’s mother, not having heard what the
child said, saw the same vision . But
this is a story at third hand. The doctor has
a number of cases, and held that crystal possesses
an ‘odylic’ quality. But a ball of
glass serves just as well as a ball of crystal, and
is much less expensive.
Children are naturally visionaries,
and, as such, are good subjects for experiment.
But it may be a cruel, and is a most injudicious
thing, to set children a-scrying. Superstition
may be excited, or the half-conscious tendency to
deceive may be put in motion.
Socrates and Joan of Arc were visionaries
as children. Had Joan’s ears been soundly
boxed, as Robert de Baudricourt advised, France might
now be an English province. But they were not
boxed, happily for mankind. Certainly much that
is curious may be learned by any one who, having the
confidence of a child, will listen to his, or her,
accounts of spontaneous visions. The writer,
as a boy, knew a child who used to lie prone on the
grass watching fairies at play in the miniature forest
of blades and leaves. This child had a favourite
familiar whom he described freely, but as his remarks
were received with good-humoured scepticism, no harm
came to him. He would have made a splendid scryer,
still, ’I speak of him but brotherly,’
his revelations would have been taken with the largest
allowances. If scrying, on examination, proves
to be of real psychological interest, science will
owe another debt to folklore, to the folk who kept
alive a practice which common-sense would not deign
even to examine.